Anonymously male: Social media avatar icons are implicitly male and resistant to change

Vol.10,No.4(2016)

Abstract
When asked to pick a typical human, people are more likely to pick a man than a woman, a phenomenon reflecting androcentrism. Social media websites provide a relevant context in which to study androcentrism since many websites aim to provide users with an ostensibly gender-neutral icon if users do not upload one of their own images. In our first study, 50 male and female online participants (Mage = 35.70) rated whether actual avatar icons from highly trafficked social media websites are perceived as gender-neutral. Using bi-polar scales from woman to man participants reported that overall the icons appeared to be more male-typed than gender-neutral. In Study 2, we investigated whether adding more female-typed icons would discourage or promote androcentric thinking. An online sample of 608 male and female participants (Mage = 33.76) viewed either 12 avatar icons that reflected the over-representation of male-typed icons or 12 that included an equal number of male and female-typed icons. Participants were then asked to produce an example of a typical person. Finally, we measured political ideology on two liberal-conservative scales. We found evidence that exposure to an equal number of male-typed and female-typed avatar icons generated reactance among political conservatives, and thus may have constituted an ideological threat. Conservatives who saw an equal number of male-typed and female-typed icons were twice as likely to come up with a man as a typical person compared to conservatives who saw an over-representation of male-typed avatar icons. Consistent with system-justification theory, these findings show how male-centric thinking is also evident in a seemingly gender-neutral online context.

Keywords:
Social media; gender; avatars; system justification theory; threat
Author biographies

April H. Bailey

April H. Bailey is a graduate student pursuing a PhD in Social Psychology at Yale University. Her research focuses on how another’s gender impacts one’s perceptions, judgments, and evaluations of them with a social cognitive orientation.

Marianne LaFrance

Marianne LaFrance is a Professor of Psychology and Women’s Gender and Sexuality Studies at Yale University. Her work broadly focuses on the psychology of gender and the role of nonverbal communication.
References

Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Sage.

American Psychological Association. (1978). Guidelines for nonsexist language in APA journals: Publication manual change sheet 2. Educational Researcher, 7(3), 15-17.

Bailey, A. H., & LaFrance, M. (2016). Who counts as human? Antecedents to androcentric behavior. Sex Roles. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-016-0648-4

Beery, T. A. (1995). Gender bias in the diagnosis and treatment of coronary artery disease. Heart & Lung: The Journal of Acute and Critical Care, 24, 427-435. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0147-9563(95)80020-4

Bem, S. L. (1993). The lenses of gender: Transforming the debate on sexual inequality. New Haven, CT: Yale University.

Bem, S. L., & Bem, D. H. (1973). Gender segregated want ads: Do they discourage female job applicants. Psychology and Social Issues. New York: Macmillan.

Broverman, I. K., Broverman, D. M., Clarkson, F. E., Rosenkrantz, P. S., & Vogel, S. R. (1970). Sex-role stereotypes and clinical judgments of mental health. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 34, 1-7. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0028797

Burke, S. E., & LaFrance, M. (2016). Lay conceptions of sexual minority groups. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 45, 635-650. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-015-0655-5

Cejka, M. A., & Eagly, A. H. (1999). Gender-stereotypic images of occupations correspond to the sex segregation of employment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 413-423. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167299025004002

Connolly, I. (2016). Self and identity in cyberspace. In I. Connolly, M. Palmer, H. Barton, & G. Kirwan (Eds.), An introduction to cyberpsychology (pp. 45-57). New York, NY: Routledge.

Cundiff, J. L. (2012). Is mainstream psychological research womanless and raceless? An updated analysis. Sex Roles, 67, 158-173. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-012-0141-7

Dasgupta, N., & Greenwald, A. G. (2001). On the malleability of automatic attitudes: Combating automatic prejudice with images of admired and disliked individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 800-814. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.81.5.800

Dubrovsky, V. J., Kiesler, S., & Sethna, B. N. (1991). The equalization phenomenon: Status effects in computer-mediated and face-to-face decision-making groups. Human-computer Interaction, 6, 119-146. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327051hci0602_2

Duggan, M., & Smith, A. (2013). Social media update 2013. Pew Research Center. Retrieved from http://www.pewinternet.org/2013/12/30/social-media-update-2013/

Dunn, O. J. (1961). Multiple comparisons among means. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 56, 52-64.

Eagly, A. H., & Karau, S. J. (2002). Role congruity theory of prejudice toward female leaders. Psychological Review, 109, 573-598. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.109.3.573

Eagly, A. H., & Kite, M. E. (1987). Are stereotypes of nationalities applied to both women and men? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 451-462. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.53.3.451

Finnegan, E., Oakhill, J., & Garnham, A. (2015). Counter-stereotypical pictures as a strategy for overcoming spontaneous gender stereotypes. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, article 1291. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01291

Forsyth, D. R., Lawrence, N. K., Burnette, J. L., & Baumeister, R. F. (2007). Attempting to improve the academic performance of struggling college students by bolstering their self-esteem: An intervention that backfired. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 26, 447-459. https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2007.26.4.447

Gao, G. (2015). Why the former USSR has far fewer men than women. Pew Research Center. Retrieved from http://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2015/08/14/why-the-former-ussr-has-far-fewer-men-than-women/

Gastil, J. (1990). Generic pronouns and sexist language: The oxymoronic character of masculine generics. Sex Roles, 23, 629-643. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf00289252

Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (2001). An ambivalent alliance: Hostile and benevolent sexism as complementary justifications for gender inequality. American Psychologist, 56, 109-118. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.56.2.109

Hallgren, K. A. (2012). Computing inter-rater reliability for observational data: An overview and tutorial. Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 8, 23-34.

Hamilton, M. C. (1991). Masculine bias in the attribution of personhood. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 15, 393-402. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.1991.tb00415.x

Hegarty, P., & Buechel, C. (2006). Androcentric reporting of gender differences in APA journals: 1965-2004. Review of General Psychology, 10, 377-389. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.10.4.377

Heilman, M. E., & Eagly, A. H. (2008). Gender stereotypes are alive, well, and busy producing workplace discrimination. Industrial and Organizational Psychology, 1, 393-398.

Hellinger, M., & Motschenbacher, H. (2015). Gender across languages (Vol. 4). Amsterdam / Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

Hyde, J. S. (1984). Children's understanding of sexist language. Developmental Psychology, 20, 697-706. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.20.4.697

Jost, J. T., & Banaji, M. R. (1994). The role of stereotyping in system-justification and the production of false consciousness. British Journal of Social Psychology, 33, 1-27. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309.1994.tb01008.x

Jost, J. T., Banaji, M. R., & Nosek, B. A. (2004). A decade of system justification theory: Accumulated evidence of conscious and unconscious bolstering of the status quo. Political Psychology, 25, 881-919. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2004.00402.x

Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. J. (2003). Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 339-375. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.129.3.339

Kay, A. C., & Friesen, J. (2011). On social stability and social change understanding when system justification does and does not occur. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 360-364. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411422059

Kay, A. C., Jost, J. T., & Young, S. (2005). Victim derogation and victim enhancement as alternate routes to system justification. Psychological Science, 16, 240-246. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00810.x

Lai, C. K., Marini, M., Lehr, S. A., Cerruti, C., Shin, J. E. L., Joy-Gaba, J. A., . . . Nosek, B. A. (2014). Reducing implicit racial preferences: I. A comparative investigation of 17 interventions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 1765-1785. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036260

Lambdin, J. R., Greer, K. M., Jibotian, K. S., Wood, K. R., & Hamilton, M. C. (2003). The animal= male hypothesis: Children's and adults' beliefs about the sex of non-sex specific stuffed animals. Sex Roles, 48, 471-482. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023567010708

Legault, L., Gutsell, J. N., & Inzlicht, M. (2011). Ironic effects of antiprejudice messages how motivational interventions can reduce (but also increase) prejudice. Psychological Science, 22, 1472–1477. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611427918

Lenhart, A. (2015). Teens, social media, and technology, overview 2015. Pew Research Center. Retrieved from < href="http://www.pewinternet.org/2015/04/09/teens-social-media-technology-2015/">http://www.pewinternet.org/2015/04/09/teens-social-media-technology-2015/

Miller, D. T., Taylor, B., & Buck, M. L. (1991). Gender gaps: Who needs to be explained? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 5-12. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.61.1.5

Miller, H., & Arnold, J. (2001). Breaking away from grounded identity? Women academics on the web. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 4, 95-108. https://doi.org/10.1089/10949310151088451

Miller, M. M., & James, L. E. (2009). Is the generic pronoun he still comprehended as excluding women? The American Journal of Psychology, 122, 483-496.

Ng, S. H. (1990). Androcentric coding of man and his in memory by language users. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 26, 455-464. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(90)90069-X

Perrin, A. (2015) Social media usage: 2005 to 2015. Pew Research Center. Retrieved from http://www.pewinternet.org/2015/10/08/social-networking-usage-2005-2015/

Prentice, D. A. (1994). Do language reforms change our way of thinking? Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 13, 3-19. https://doi.org/10.1177/0261927X94131001

Reis, H. T., & Carothers, B. J. (2014). Black and white or shades of gray are gender differences categorical or dimensional? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23, 19-26. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721413504105

Roussos, G. & Dovidio, J. F. (2016). Playing below the poverty line: Investigating an online game as a way to reduce prejudice toward the poor. Cyberpsychology: Journal of Psychosocial Research on Cyberspace, 10, article 3. https://doi.org/10.5817/cp2016-2-3

Schneider, J. W., & Hacker, S. L. (1973). Sex role imagery and use of the generic "man" in introductory texts: A case in the sociology of sociology. The American Sociologist, 8, 12-18.

Sczesny, S., Formanowicz, M., & Moser, F. (2016). Can gender-fair language reduce gender stereotyping and discrimination? Frontiers in Psychology, 7, article 25. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00025

Sczesny, S., Moser, F., & Wood, W. (2015). Beyond sexist beliefs: How do people decide to use gender-inclusive language? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41, 943-54. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167215585727

Silveira, J. (1980). Generic masculine words and thinking. Women's Studies International Quarterly, 3, 165-178. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0148-0685(80)92113-2

Stout, J. G., & Dasgupta, N. (2011). When he doesn’t mean you: Gender-exclusive language as ostracism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37, 757-769. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167211406434

Switzer, J. Y. (1990). The impact of generic word choices: An empirical investigation of age-and sex-related differences. Sex Roles, 22, 69-82. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00288155

Vorauer, J. D., & Sasaki, S. J. (2009). Helpful only in the abstract? Ironic effects of empathy in intergroup interaction. Psychological Science, 20, 191-197.

Metrics

web of science logo


6072

Views

2383

PDF views