The quality of online, offline, and mixed-mode friendships among users of a social networking site
Vol.6,No.3(2012)
social networking sites; quality of friendship; similarity; proximity; social attraction
Marjolijn L. Antheunis
Tilburg School of Humanities, Tilburg University, Tilburg, The Netherlands
Marjolijn L. Antheunis (Ph.D., University of Amsterdam, the Netherlands) is an Assistant Professor at Tilburg Center for Cognition and Communication, TiCC at Tilburg University. Her research interests focus on the social aspects of new media and the effects of online communication on friendship formation and friendship quality.
Patti M. Valkenburg
Amsterdam School of Communication Research, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Patti M. Valkenburg (Ph.D., Leiden University, the Netherlands) is a Professor in the Amsterdam School of Communication Research, ASCoR, at the University of Amsterdam. Her research interests focus on the effects of media on the cognitive, affective, and social development of children and adolescents.
Jochen Peter
Amsterdam School of Communication Research, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Jochen Peter (Ph.D., University of Amsterdam, the Netherlands) is a Professor in the Amsterdam School of Communication Research, ASCoR, at the University of Amsterdam. His research focuses on the consequences of adolescents' media use for their sexual socialization and psycho-social development. Specifically, he investigates the effects of teenagers' use of online sexually explicit material on their sexual attitudes and behaviors.
Aboud, F. E., & Mendelson, M. J. (1996). Determinants of friendship selection and quality: Developmental perspectives. In W. M. Bukowski, A. F. Newcomb & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), The company they keep: Friendship in childhood and adolescence (pp. 87-112). Cambridge: University Press.
Adler, P., & Kwon, S. (2002). Social capital: Prospects for a new concept. Academy of Management Review, 27, 17-40. https://doi.org/10.5465/amr.2002.5922314
Albada, K. F., Knapp, M. L., & Theune, K. E. (2002). Interaction appearance theory: Changing perceptions of physical attractiveness through social interaction. Communication Theory, 12, 8-40. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2885.2002.tb00257.x
Antheunis, M. L., Valkenburg, P. M., & Peter, J. (2007). Computer-mediated communication and interpersonal attraction: An experimental test of two explanatory hypotheses. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 10, 831-836. https://doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2007.9945
Bargh, J. A., McKenna, K. Y., & Fitzsimons, G. M. (2002). Can you see the real me? Activation and expression of the "true self" on the Internet. Journal of Social Issues, 58, 33-48. https://doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2007.9945
Berndt, T. J., Hawkins, J. A., & Hoyle, S. G. (1986). Changes in friendship during a school year: Effects of children's and adolescents' impressions of friendship and sharing with friends. Child Development, 57, 1284-1297. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130451
Berscheid, E., & Walster, E. (1969). Interpersonal attraction. Reading, Mass: Addison-Wesley.
Bourdieu, P., & Wacquant, L. (1992). An invitation to reflexive sociology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
boyd, d. m. (2004, April). Friendster and publicly articulated social networking. Paper presented at the Conference on human factors and computing systems, Vienna.
boyd, d. m., & Ellison, N. B. (2008). Social network sites: Definition, history, and scholarship. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 13, 210-230. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1083-6101.2007.00393.x
Brehm, S. (1992). Intimate relationships. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Burgoon, J. K., Bonito, J. A., Ramirez, A., Dunbar, N. E., Kam, K., & Fischer, J. (2002). Testing the interactivity principle: Effects of mediation, propinquity, and verbal and nonverbal modalities in interpersonal interaction. Journal of Communication, 52, 657-677. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2002.tb02567.x
Chan, D. K.-S., & Cheng, G. H.-L. (2004). A comparison of offline and online friendship qualities at different stages of relationship development. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 21, 305-320. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407504042834
Clark, M., & Pataki, S. (1995). Interpersonal processes influencing attraction and relationships. In A. Tesser (Ed.), Advanced social psychology (pp. 283–331). St. Louis: McGraw Hill.
Collins, N. L., & Miller, L. C. (1994). Self-disclosure and liking: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 457-475. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.116.3.457
ComScore, Inc (2007). Social networking goes global: major social networking sites substantially expanded their global visitor base during past year, Retrieved from http://www.comscore.com/press/release.asp?press=1555
Cooper, A., & Sportolari, L. (1997). Romance in cyberspace: Understanding online attraction. Journal of Sex Education and Therapy, 22, 7-14. https://doi.org/10.1080/01614576.1997.11074165
Donath, J. S., & boyd, d. m. (2004). Public displays of connection. BT Technology Journal, 22, 71. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:BTTJ.0000047585.06264.cc
Dubrovsky, V. J., Kiesler, S., & Sethna, B. N. (1991). The equalization phenomenon: Status effects in computer-mediated and face-to-face decision-making groups. Human-computer Interaction, 6, 119-146. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327051hci0602_2
Duck, S. W. (1975). Personality similarity and friendship choices by adolescents. European journal of social psychology, 5, 351-365. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2420050308
Duck, S. W. (1983). Friends for life. Brighton, UK: Harvester.
Dwyer, C., Hiltz, S. R., & Passerini, K. (2007, August). Trust and privacy concern within social networking sites: A comparison of Facebook and MySpace. Paper presented at the Thirteenth Americas Conference on Information Systems, Keystone, Colorado.
Ellison, N. B., Steinfield, C., & Lampe, C. (2006, June). Spatially bounded online social networks and social capital: The role of Facebook. Paper presented at the 56th Annual Conference of the International Communication Association, Dresden.
Ellison, N. B., Steinfield, C., & Lampe, C. (2007). The benefits of Facebook "friends": Social capital and college students' use of online social network sites. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 12, 1143-1168. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1083-6101.2007.00367.x
Grannovetter, M. S. (1983). The strength of weak ties: A network theory revisited. Sociological Theory, 1, 201-233. https://doi.org/10.2307/202051
Gross, E. F. (2004). Adolescent Internet use: What we expect, what teens report. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 25, 633-649. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2004.09.005
Hallinan, M. T., & Kubitschek, W. N. (1988). The effects of individual and structural characteristics on intransivity in social networks. Social Psychology Quarterly, 51, 81-92. https://doi.org/10.2307/2786832
Hays, R. B. (1985). A longitudinal study of friendship development. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 909-924. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.48.4.909
Kandel, D. B. (1978). Homophily, selection, and socialization in adolescent friendship. The American Journal of Sociology, 84, 427-436. https://doi.org/10.1086/226792
Kohut, A. (2008). Internet's broader role in campaign 2008: Social networking and online videos take off. Washington, DC: Pew Research Centre.
Lampe, C., Ellison, N. B., & Steinfield, C. (2006, November). A Face(book): in the crowd: Social searching vs. social browsing. Paper presented at the 20th anniversary conference on Computer Supported Cooperative Work, Banff, Alberta, Canada.
Lazarsfeld, P. F., & Merton, R. K. (1954). Friendship as social process: A substantive and methodological analysis. In M. Berger, T. Abel & C. H. Page (Eds.), Freedom and control in modern society. Toronto, Canada: Nostrand.
Lea, M., & Spears, R. (1992). Paralanguage and social perception in computer-mediated communication. Journal of Organizational Computing, 2, 321-341. https://doi.org/10.1080/10919399209540190
Marsden, P. V., & Campbell, K. E. (1984). Measuring tie strength. Social Forces, 63, 482-501. https://doi.org/10.1093/sf/63.2.482
McCroskey, J. C., & McCain, T. A. (1974). The measurement of interpersonal attraction. Speech Monograph, 41, 261-266. https://doi.org/10.1080/03637757409375845
McCroskey, J. C., Richmond, V. P., & Daly, J. A. (1975). The development of a measure of perceived homophily in interpersonal communication. Human Communication Research, 1, 323-332. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.1975.tb00281.x
McKenna, K. Y., & Bargh, J. A. (2000). Plan 9 from cyberspace: The implications of the Internet for personality and social psychology. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4, 57-75.
McKenna, K. Y., Green, A. S., & Gleason, M. E. (2002). Relationship formation on the Internet: What's the big attraction? Journal of Social Issues, 58, 9-31. https://doi.org/10.1111/1540-4560.00246
McLuhan, M., & Powers, B. R. (1989). The global village: Transformations in world life and media in the 21st century. New York: Oxford University Press.
McPherson, M., Smith-Lovin, L., & Cook, J. M. (2001). Birds of a feather: Homophily in social networks. Annual Review of Sociology, 27, 415–444. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.soc.27.1.415
Mesch, G. S., & Talmud, I. (2006). The quality of online and offline relationships: The role of multiplexity and duration of social relationships. The Information Society, 22, 137-148. https://doi.org/10.1080/01972240600677805
Mesch, G. S., & Talmud, I. (2007). Similarity and the quality of online and offline social relationships among adolescents in Israel. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 17, 455-466. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7795.2007.00529.x
Newcomb, T. (1961). The acquaintance process. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
Oswald, D. L., & Clark, E. M. (2003). Best friends forever?: High school best friendships and the transition to college. Personal Relationships, 10, 187-196. https://doi.org/10.1111/1475-6811.00045
Parks, M. R., & Floyd, K. (1996). Making friends in cyberspace. Journal of Communication, 46, 80-97. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.1996.tb01462.x
Parks, M. R., & Roberts, L. D. (1998). 'Making MOOsic': The development of personal relationships on line and a comparison to their off-line counterparts. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 15, 517-537. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407598154005
Preciado, P., Snijders, T. A. B., Burke, W. J., Stattin, H., & Kerr, M. (2012). Does proximity matter? Distance dependence of adolescent friendships. Social Networks, 34, 18-31.
Priest, R. F., & Sawyer, J. (1967). Proximity and peership: Bases of balance in interpersonal attraction. The American Journal of Sociology, 72, 633-649. https://doi.org/10.1086/224400
Putnam, R. (2000). Bowling alone: The collapse and revival of American community. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Ramirez, A., & Zhang, S. (2007). When online meets offline: The effect of modality switching on relational communication. Communication Monographs, 74, 287-310. https://doi.org/10.1080/03637750701543493
Reagans, R. (2005). Preferences, identity, and competition: Predicting tie strength from demographic data. Management Science, 51, 1374-1383. https://doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.1050.0389
Reis, H. T., & Shaver, P. (1988). Intimacy as an interpersonal process. In S. Duck (Ed.), Handbook of personal relationships (pp. 367-390). New York: Wiley.
Spears, R., & Lea, M. (1992). Social influence and the influence of the "social" in computer-mediated communication. In M. Lea (Ed.), Contexts of computer-mediated communication (pp. 30-65). Hemel Hempstead: Harvester Wheatsheaf.
Steinfield, C., Ellison, N. B., & Lampe, C. (2008). Social capital, self-esteem, and use of online social network sites: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 29, 434-445. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2008.07.002
Tidwell, L. C., & Walther, J. B. (2002). Computer-mediated communication effects on disclosure, impressions, and interpersonal evaluations: Getting to know one another a bit at a time. Human Communication Research, 28, 317-348. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.2002.tb00811.x
Utz, S. (2000). Social information processing in MUDs: The development of friendships in virtual worlds. Journal of Online Behavior, 1, 1.
VanAlstyne, M., & Brynjolfsson, E. (1996, December). Electronic communities: Global village or cyberbalkans? Paper presented at the 17th International Conference on Information Systems, Cleveland, OH.
Vittengl, J. R., & Holt, G. S. (2000). Getting acquainted: the relationship of self-disclosure and social attraction to positive affect. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 17, 53-66. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407500171003
Walther, J. B. (1992). Interpersonal effects in computer-mediated interaction; A relational perspective. Communication Research, 19, 52-90. https://doi.org/10.1177/009365092019001003
Walther, J. B. (1995). Relational aspects of computer-mediated communication: Experimental observations over time. Organization Science, 6, 186-203. https://doi.org/10.1287/orsc.6.2.186
Walther, J. B. (1996). Computer-mediated communication: Impersonal, interpersonal, and hyperpersonal interaction. Communication Research, 23, 3-43. https://doi.org/10.1177/009365096023001001
Walther, J. B., & Parks, M. R. (2002). Cues filtered out, cues filtered in; computer mediated communication and relationships. In M. L. Knapp & J. A. Daly (Eds.), Handbook of interpersonal communication (3rd ed., pp. 529-561). Thousand Oaks, Ca: Sage.
Wellman, B., & Gulia, M. (1997). Net surfers don't ride alone: Virtual communities as communities. In P. Kollock & M. Smith (Eds.), Communities and cyberspace. New York: Routledge.
Williams, D. (2006). On and off the ‘net’: Scales for social capital in an online era. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 11, 593-628. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1083-6101.2006.00029.x
Wolak, J., Mitchell, K. J., & Finkelhor, D. (2003). Escaping or connecting? Characteristics of youth who form close online relationships. Journal of Adolescence, 26, 105-119. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-1971(02)00114-8
Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Monograph, 9, 1-27. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0025848
Introduction
Social capital is important in society. It has been linked to a variety of positive social outcomes, such as better public health, lower crime rates, and more efficient financial markets (Adler & Kwon, 2002). Social capital can be defined as “the sum of resources, actual or virtual, that accrue to an individual or a group by virtue of possessing a durable network of more or less institutionalized relationships of mutual acquaintance an recognition” (Bourdieu & Wacquant, 1992, p.14). Putnam (2000) distinguishes between two types of social capital: bridging and bonding.
Bridging social capital is the benefit from weak ties, found between individuals with loose connections who may provide useful information, but typically not emotional support (Putnam, 2000; Grannovetter, 1983). Bonding social capital is the benefit from strong ties, found between individuals in emotionally close and high quality relationships, such as family and close friends (Putnam, 2000). Although there is ample research about the effect of the Internet on bridging social capital (Donath & boyd, 2004; Ellison, Steinfield, & Lampe, 2007; Steinfield, Ellison, & Lampe, 2008), little empirical work has examined the effects of the Internet on bonding social capital (Williams, 2006).
It is unclear how social capital formation occurs when online and offline connections are closely coupled, as is the case with social networking sites (Ellison et al., 2007). Williams (2006) argued that although researchers have examined potential losses of social capital in offline communities due to increase Internet use, they have not adequately explored online gains that might compensate for this (Ellison et al., 2007). For example, Ellison et al. (2007) found that the use of social networking sites is positively related to the individuals’ perceived bonding social capital. Although it is important to examine the effect of social networking site usage on social capital, it is also important to examine the quality of the friendships on social networking sites. After all, the quality of the friendship (i.e., strength of tie) affects the capital it yields. Therefore, in this study, we focus on the quality of different friendships on social networking sites.
Social networking sites are typically used both to keep in touch with existing friends and to develop new friendships (boyd, 2004; Dwyer, Hiltz, & Passerini, 2007; Ellison et al., 2006). Social networking sites, therefore, distinguish themselves from other interpersonal Computer-Mediated Communication (CMC) applications, such as e-mail, or Instant Messaging. Both of which are predominantly used to either communicate with unknown people, or to maintain relationships with one’s existing friends (e.g., Gross, 2004). Thus, social networking sites provide a good opportunity to investigate and compare both the frequency and nature of different types of friendships. On social networking sites, three types of friendships can be distinguished: (a) online friendships, which are developed online and remain solely online; (b) mixed-mode friendships (Walther & Parks, 2002), which are developed online but have extended to other, offline settings; and (c) offline friendships, which are developed offline and have extended to online settings.
One of the key features of friendships is their quality. The quality of friendships refers to the experienced closeness, trust, and understanding between friends (Marsden & Campbell, 1984). Several studies have investigated and compared the quality of online versus offline friendships (Chan & Cheng, 2004; Mesch & Talmud, 2006, 2007; Parks & Roberts, 1998). These studies have consistently demonstrated that online friendships are perceived to be lower in quality than offline friendships (Mesch & Talmud, 2006, 2007; Parks & Roberts, 1998). Furthermore, Chan and Cheng (2004) found that, although the quality of both online and offline friendships increased over time, the quality of online friendships improved significantly more than offline relationships. Specifically, they discovered when online friendships lasted for more than a year, their quality became comparable to offline friendships.
Although these earlier studies have yielded rather consistent results, they are limited by their unclear definitions of online and offline friendships. The studies usually referred to online friendships as originating online and to offline friendships as originating offline (Mesch & Talmud, 2006, 2007; Parks & Roberts, 1998). However, in our view, this binary distinction does not reflect the reality of contemporary friendship formation and maintenance. After all, it has been demonstrated that a considerable part of online friendships migrate to offline settings in the course of their development and, as a result, develop into mixed-mode friendships (Parks & Floyd, 1996; Parks & Roberts, 1998; Wolak, Mitchell, & Finkelhor, 2003). As earlier studies did not take this possible confound into account, they may not have compared differences between online and offline friendships, but rather differences between mixed-mode and offline friendships.
To our knowledge, no earlier research has compared the perceived quality of mixed-mode friendship with that of online and offline friendships. Therefore, the first aim of this study is to fill this lacuna in the literature:
RQ1a: Is there a difference in friendship quality between online, mixed-mode, and offline friendships?
Duration of the Friendship as a Moderator
Earlier research suggests that potential differences in the quality between online, mixed-mode, and offline friendships may depend on the duration of the relationship. For example, Walther’s (1992) social information processing theory, assumes that relationship formation in CMC occurs at a slower rate than in face-to-face settings. According to Walther (1995), CMC does not prevent the development of close relationships but, due to the reduced nonverbal cues, rather it slows down the process. Thus, social information processing theory implies that the quality of online and mixed-mode friendships may be particularly affected by the duration of the friendship.
Based on the results of Chan and Cheng (2004), we assume that the quality of online and mixed-mode friendships will initially be lower than that of offline friendships, but that these differences decrease when online and mixed-mode relationships develop over time. However, to date this assumption has not been examined for online, mixed-mode, and offline friendships. Therefore, our next research question reads:
RQ1b: Will potential differences in the quality between online, mixed-mode, and offline friendships become smaller as the friendships develop over time?
Determinants of the Quality of Online, Mixed-mode, and Offline Friendships
A second aim of our study is to investigate and compare the factors that predict the quality of online, mixed-mode, and offline friendships. Theories of offline friendships have yielded several factors that determine the quality of offline friendships. Three determinants have received particular attention in the literature: (a) proximity (e.g., Hays, 1985; Priest & Sawyer, 1967), (b) similarity (e.g., Duck, 1975; Kandel, 1978; Reagans, 2005; Wellman & Gulia, 1997), and (c) social attraction (e.g., Berndt, Hawkins, & Hoyle, 1986; Reagans, 2005). Proximity refers to the geographic closeness between friends (Burgoon et al., 2002). Similarity, in this study, refers to perceived attitudinal similarity, that is, the extent to which friends feel similar to each other with respect to their attitudes. Social attraction is the socio-emotional part of interpersonal attraction and is sometimes also called ‘liking’. Social attraction implies that a target person is pleasant to be with, could become a friend, and would fit in the existing circle of friends (McCroskey & McCain, 1974).
In our study, we first investigate whether the level of proximity, perceived similarity, and social attraction differs among online, mixed-mode, and offline friendships. Subsequently, we compare the relative contribution of each of these three determinants in the prediction of the quality of the three types of friendships. To our knowledge, no earlier research has compared the relative importance of these three determinants for the quality of online, mixed-mode, and offline friendships. We therefore abstain from formulating hypotheses and investigate research questions instead.
Proximity. Research on offline friendships has shown that the closer the physical distance between subjects and their target friends, the more likely the friendship will be a success (Hays, 1985; Mesch & Talmud, 2007; Priest & Sawyer, 1967). Geographically close people become friends more easily, not only because they can interact more easily (e.g., Clark & Pataki, 1995), but also because they have more opportunity to exchange information between one another, have mutual peer groups, and participate in joint activities (Oswald & Clark, 2003). As a result, they are more likely to become close to each other (Newcomb, 1961; McPherson, Smith-Lovin, & Cook, 2001; Preciado, Snijders, Burk, Stattin, & Kerr, 2012).
The global-village metaphor (e.g., McLuhan & Power, 1989), proposes the Internet globalizes communication by allowing users from around the world to connect with each other. Based on this, it is plausible to assume that proximity is less important in online and mixed-mode friendships than in offline friendships (e.g., McKenna & Bargh, 2000). Through the Internet, one can meet people from all over the world. Consequently, the chances of meeting someone who lives nearby may be smaller on the Internet than in offline settings.
However, it has also been suggested that CMC may stimulate feelings of proximity between interaction partners, regardless of their actual geographic distance (Cooper & Sportolari, 1997). These virtual feelings of proximity have been explained with Zajonc’s (1968) mere exposure effect (e.g., McKenna & Bargh, 2000). The mere exposure effect implies that repeated exposure to an object elicits positive feelings toward this object. Based on the mere exposure effect, it can be predicted that even without being geographically close, frequent contact with online communication partners leads to feelings of proximity between these partners. As a result, actual geographical proximity may be less important for the quality of online friendships and perhaps also for the quality of mixed-mode friendships.
Empirical evidence on the relationship between the geographical proximity of friends and the quality of friendship is mixed. In one study, Mesch and Talmud (2006) found that proximity did not predict the quality of online friendships, whereas in another study, it did affect the quality of online friendships (Mesch & Talmud, 2007). Unfortunately, they do not give an explanation for their conflicting findings. Therefore, our next research questions read:
RQ2a: How does the level of geographical proximity differ for online, mixed-mode, and offline friendships?
RQ2b: What is the relative contribution of geographical proximity to the quality of online, mixed-mode, and offline friendships?
Similarity. Researchers agree that the more similar two people are, the stronger their relationship (Duck, 1983; Hallinan & Kubitschek, 1988; Kandel, 1978; Lazarsfeld & Merton, 1954; Mesch & Talmud, 2007; Reagans, 2005). People are more likely to participate in joint activities with others who share their interests. By doing so, they receive validation of their attitudes and beliefs, which they perceive as rewarding (Aboud & Mendelson, 1996). These perceived rewards, in turn, stimulate further interactions and thereby, the formation and maintenance of friendships (Reagans, 2005). Therefore, it is no surprise that friendships between similar people are usually of a higher quality than between dissimilar people (Mesch & Talmud, 2007).
Several CMC theories have suggested that CMC partners feel more similar than offline communication partners. For example, the hyperpersonal communication framework (Walther, 1996) argues that in the absence of nonverbal cues, CMC interactants tend to focus on those cues that are available. Therefore, they will engage in an “over-attribution” process, leading to idealized impressions of each other. Furthermore, social identity deindividuation (SIDE) theory (Lea & Spears, 1992; Spears & Lea, 1992) states that when social cues are absent, group identity replaces individual identity in CMC, and therefore interactants feel more similar. Both theories imply that online communication partners have less access to nonverbal cues, such as clothing and mimicry, which often uncover interpersonal differences in face-to-face settings. Online partners are thus forced to focus on whatever minimal cues appear in an online setting. In the absence of nonverbal cues, which typically prevent feelings of similarity, this over-reliance on available cues may lead online partners to easily feel similar to their communication partners.
The assumption that CMC increases feelings of similarity has received empirical support (Dubrovsky, Kiesler, & Sethna, 1991). However, this CMC-enhanced perceived similarity may not hold true for CMC on social networking sites. Social networking sites provide more cues than text-based CMC, especially through the availability of non-verbal cues. For example, social networking sites allow photos to be posted, which may effectively impair the development of feelings of similarity. As the literature does not allow us to formulate hypotheses on the effects of similarity on the three types of friendships, we investigate the following two research questions:
RQ3a: How does the level of perceived similarity differ between online, mixed-mode, and offline friendships?
RQ3b: What is the relative contribution of perceived similarity to the quality of online, mixed-mode, and offline friendships?
Social attraction. Social attraction is one of the most important determinants of the quality of offline friendships (Aboud & Mendelson, 1996; Berndt et al., 1986; Reagans, 2005). The formation of offline relationships does not occur without at least a minimum level of social attraction (Brehm, 1992; Reagans, 2005; Reis & Shaver, 1988). People prefer to interact with others to whom they feel socially attracted, because they are more pleasant to be with (Vittengl & Holt, 2000). As a result, socially attracted people are more likely to communicate with each other (Berscheid & Walster, 1969; McCroskey & McCain, 1974), which eventually may result in a higher level of friendship quality (Reagans, 2005).
The level of social attraction may be higher in online friendships than in offline friendships. A series of studies have reported positive effects of CMC on social attraction (Antheunis, Valkenburg, & Peter, 2007; Bargh, McKenna, & Fitzsimons, 2002; McKenna, Green, & Gleason, 2002; Ramirez & Zhang, 2007). These positive effects have been explained by the use of two related interactive uncertainty reduction strategies in CMC: self-disclosure and direct questioning (Albada, Knapp, & Theune, 2002; Bargh et al., 2002; Collins & Miller, 1994; McKenna et al., 2002). CMC stimulates the use of both self-disclosure and direct questioning (Antheunis et al., 2007; Tidwell & Walther, 2002). CMC-enhanced self-disclosure and direct questioning, in turn, stimulate social attraction (Antheunis et al., 2007).
The finding that CMC enhances interpersonal attraction is based on research in CMC environments with no or limited non-verbal cues. As a result, it may be conceivable that interpersonal attraction may develop differently on social networking sites. As discussed above, users of social networking sites also have non-verbal cues at their disposal. Consequently, they do not have to rely predominantly on self-disclosure and direct questioning to reduce their uncertainty about other communication partners. Eventually, then, the availability of a greater number of cues on social networking sites may reduce the development of social attraction. To our knowledge, the effect of CMC induced social attraction on the quality of friendships has not been investigated. Our last two research questions related to social attraction read:
RQ4a: How does the level of social attraction differ for online, mixed-mode, and offline friendships?
RQ4b: What is the relative contribution of social attraction to the quality of online, mixed-mode, and offline friendships?
Method
Sample and Procedure
In 2007, an online survey was conducted among 2,188 active members of Hyves1 (30.2 % male). The average age was 22.95 (SD = 5.96). The participants were recruited through advertisements on the Hyves website. After a brief introduction, participants were asked some general questions about their use of Hyves. Subsequently, they were randomly questioned about either their online or their offline friendships. If respondents were questioned about online friendships, we asked them whether they had one or more friends on Hyves who they had met through Hyves or the Internet. It did not matter where the online friends on Hyves met each other, as long as it was online. If they were questioned about offline friendships, we asked them whether they had one or more friends on Hyves who they already knew offline. In both situations, respondents were asked to think of the particular online or offline friend with whom they had communicated with most recently through Hyves. The participants were then asked a series of questions about this particular friend. If the participant in the online friendship condition had not met one friend through Hyves or the Internet, the questionnaire was ended.
To classify the friendships in online, mixed-mode, and offline friendships, we asked the respondents which channels they had used to communicate with the particular friend. The response categories were: (a) Hyves, (b) Instant Messaging (IM), (c) email, (d) telephone, and (e) face-to-face. If respondents had met their friend through Hyves or the Internet and had used only Hyves, IM, and/or email to communicate with this particular friend, the friend was identified as an online friend (N = 357). If respondents had also used telephone and/or face-to-face communication, the online friend was classified as a mixed-mode friend (N = 347). Finally, if respondents indicated that they already knew their friend from offline settings, the friend was identified as an offline friend (N = 1,484).
Measures
Proximity. To measure proximity, we asked the respondents to indicate how far they lived from the particular friend. The response categories were: (1) more than 100 kilometers (i.e., about 62 miles), (2) between 50 and 100 kilometers (i.e., between 31 and 62 miles), (3) between 20 and 50 kilometers (i.e., between 12 and 31 miles), (4) between 10 and 20 kilometers (i.e., between 6 and 12 miles), (5) between 5 and 10 kilometers (i.e., between 3 and 6 miles), and (6) less than 5 kilometers (i.e., about 3 miles) (M = 3.69, SD = 1.83).
Social Attraction. Our measure of social attraction was based on McCroskey and McCain’s Measurement of Interpersonal Attraction (1974). The items were: “This person is pleasant to talk with,” “This person is pleasant to be with,” “This person fits well in my circle of friends,” “We get along well together,” “It feels comfortable when he/she is around.” The response categories ranged from 1 (completely disagree) to 5 (completely agree). The five items formed a one-dimensional scale (explained variance 79%), with a Cronbach’s alpha of .93 (M = 3.67, SD = 1.04).
Similarity. To measure similarity, we used four items of the Perceived Homophily Measure (McCroskey, Richmond, & Daly, 1975): “This person thinks like me,” “This person behaves like me,” “This person is similar to me,” and “This person is like me.” The response categories for each of the items ranged from 1 (completely disagree) to 5 (completely agree). The items formed a one-dimensional scale (explained variance 78%), with a Cronbach’s alpha of .90 (M = 2.71, SD = 1.03).
Quality of Friendship. To measure the quality of friendship, we used the items of Marsden and Campbell’s (1984) scale to measure tie strength. The four items were: “I feel close to this person,” “This person is important for me,” “If I had a personal problem, I would ask this person for help,” and “I trust this person.” The response categories for each of the items ranged from 1 (completely disagree) to 5 (completely agree). The four items loaded on one factor (explained variance 81%), and resulted in a Cronbach’s alpha of .92 (M = 3.20, SD = 1.22).
Duration of the friendship. The duration of the friendship was measured by an open question asking the respondents to indicate how long they had known this particular friend (M = 53.0 months, SD = 68.0). The variable was positively skewed (2.10). To compare the quality of friendship at different stages of the relation, we recoded the open answers, based on Chan and Cheng (2004), within five groups: 0-3 months, 4-6 months, 7-12 months, 13-24 months, and more than 24 months.
Results
Descriptive Results
The sample consisted of 357 online friendships, 347 mixed-mode friendships, and 1,484 offline friendships. The average age of the respondents in the online friendships group (67.8% female) was 22.6 (SD = 6.5). The mean duration of the online friendships was 10.97 months (SD = 24.4) and the mean distance between the online friends was 2.8 kilometres (SD = 1.61), around 1.13 miles. The average age of the respondents in the mixed-mode friendship group (61.7 % female) was 22.31 (SD = 6.57). The mean duration of the mixed-mode friendships was 17.35 months (SD = 22.3) and the mean distance between the mixed-mode friends was 3.51 kilometres (SD = 1.70), around 2.32 miles. In the offline friendship group (72.2% female) the average age was 23.18 (SD = 5.65). The mean duration of the offline friendships was 71.44 months (SD = 74.2) and the mean distance between the offline friends was 3.94 kilometres (SD = 1.85), around 2.78 miles.
Our first research question (RQ1a) asked if there is a difference in the quality of friendship between online, mixed-mode, and offline friendships. Our next research question (RQ1b) asked whether potential differences in the quality between online, mixed-mode, and offline friendships would become smaller over time. To answer these research questions, we conducted a univariate ANOVA with the quality of friendship as the dependent variable and the three types of friendship (online vs. mixed-mode vs. offline) and duration of the relationship (0-3 months vs. 4-6 months vs. 7-12 months vs. 13-24 months vs. more than 24 months) as the independent variables.
Table 1. Mean Quality of Online, Mixed-Mode, and Offline Friendships Over Time.
|
Quality of Friendship |
||||||
Type of friendship |
|
Overall |
0-3 Months |
4-6 Months |
7-12 Months |
13-24 Months |
24+ Months |
Online (n = 357) |
M(SD) % |
1.96 (0.91) a 100 |
1.86 (0.85) a 47 |
2.04 (1.00) a 22 |
1.70 (0.72) a 14 |
2.26 (0.83) a 8 |
2.41 (1.13) a 9 |
Mixed-Mode (n = 347) |
M(SD) % |
3.27 (0.99) b 100 |
3.00 (0.99) b 24 |
2.99 (1.04) b 21 |
3.31 (0.96) b 19 |
3.48 (0.92) b 15 |
3.68 (0.81) b 21 |
Offline (n = 1484) |
M(SD) % |
3.49 (0.95) c 100 |
3.02 (0.93) b 7 |
3.03 (1.01) b 5 |
3.32 (0.98) b 7 |
3.41 (0.88) b 13 |
3.61 (0.92) b 68 |
Note. % Represents row percentages of the occurrence of the particular type of friendship in this period. Column entries with different superscripts differ from each other at least at p < .001. |
The ANOVA yielded a main effect of type of friendship on the quality of friendship, F (2, 2173) = 169.99, p < .001, η2 = .13. Bonferroni post-hoc test revealed that the quality of offline friendships was significantly higher than the quality of mixed-mode and online friendships. Though lower than the quality of offline friendships, the quality of mixed-mode friendships was higher than that of online friendships (means and standard deviations are listed in Table 1). We also found a main effect of relationship duration on the quality of friendship, F (4, 2173) = 16.78, p < .001, η2 = .03. As Table 1 shows, the quality of friendship indeed improved as the friendship developed over time, regardless of the type of friendship.
The ANOVA did not yield an interaction effect between type and duration of the friendship, F (8, 2173) = 1.51, p = .15, η2 = .01, as found by Chan and Cheng (2004). Bonferroni post-hoc test showed that the quality of all three types of friendships increased over time. Although the quality of offline and mixed-mode friendships differed when the overall sample was compared (see third column of Table 1), the quality of these two types of friendship did not differ when compared over time. However, the significant differences between the quality of online friendships compared to mixed-mode and offline friendships remained, even for friendships lasting longer than two years.
Proximity
Our second research question (RQ2a) asked whether the level of geographical proximity between friends differ for offline, mixed-mode, and online friendships. The first column of Table 2 shows significant differences in the level of proximity between the types of friendship, F (1, 2185) = 61.09, p < .001. Bonferroni post-hoc test revealed that proximity was highest in offline friendships (M = 3.94, SE = 1.85), followed by mixed-mode (M = 3.51, SE = 1.70), and online friendships (M = 2.80, SE = 1.61). This means that the actual distance between offline friends is the lowest, followed by mixed-mode and online friends.
Table 2. Mean Differences in Proximity, Similarity, and Social Attraction in Online, Mixed-Mode, and Offline Friendships.
Type of friendship |
Proximity# |
Similarity |
Social Attraction |
Online |
2.80 (1.61) a |
2.06 (0.89) a |
2.55 (0.99) a |
Mixed-mode |
3.51 (1.70) b |
2.92 (0.91) b |
3.74 (0.78) b |
Offline |
3.94 (1.85) c |
2.82 (0.84) b |
3.93 (0.71) c |
Note. Column entries with different superscripts differ from each other at least at p < .001. # A higher score on proximity means a higher geographical closeness. |
Our next research question (RQ2b) asked if the effect of proximity on quality of friendship differ in offline, mixed-mode, and online friendships. To answer this and the following research questions, we conducted a multiple regression analysis for each type of friendship (online, mixed-mode, and offline), with proximity, similarity, social attraction, duration of the relationship, age, and gender as the independent variables and the quality of friendship as the dependent variable. As the first row in Table 3 shows, proximity did not affect the quality of friendship in any of the three types of friendships.
Similarity
Our third research question (RQ3a) asked if there is a difference in perceived similarity between online, mixed-mode, and offline friendships. As the second column of Table 2 shows, there is a significant difference in perceived similarity between the online and mixed-mode friendships, and between online and offline friendships, F (1, 2185) = 125.58, p <.001. Perceived similarity was the highest in mixed-mode friendships (M = 2.92, SE = 0.91) and offline friendships (M = 2.82, SE = 0.84), and lowest in online friendships (M = 2.06, SE = 0.89). Bonferroni post-hoc test revealed that there was no difference in perceived similarity between mixed-mode and offline friendships. However, the differences between mixed-mode and online friendships, and between offline and online friendships were significant.
Research question RQ3b asked whether there are different effects of perceived similarity on the quality of friendship in online, mixed-mode, and offline friendships. The results of our regression analyses showed that perceived similarity affected the quality of friendship in all three types of friendship (see Table 3). The largest effect occurred for online friendships, β = 0.51, p < .001, followed by mixed-mode friendships, β = 0.24, p < .001, and offline friendships, β = 0.19, p < .001.
Social Attraction
Our fourth research question (RQ4a) asked whether the level of social attraction differed in online, mixed-mode, and offline friendships. As the third column of Table 2 shows, there was a difference in the level of social attraction in online, mixed-mode, and offline friendships, F (1,2185) = 454.14, p<.001. Bonferroni post-hoc test showed that social attraction was the highest in offline friendships (M = 3.93, SE = 0.71), followed by mixed-mode friendships (M = 3.74, SE = 0.78) and online friendships (M = 2.55, SE = 0.99).
Table 3. Predictors and Correlations of the Quality of Online, Mixed-Mode, and Offline Friendships.
Quality of friendship |
||||||
|
Online |
Mixed-mode |
Offline |
|||
|
β |
r |
β |
r |
β |
r |
Proximity |
-0.01 |
-.05 |
-0.01 |
-.03 |
0.01 |
.11*** |
Similarity |
0.51*** |
.78*** |
0.24*** |
.66*** |
0.19*** |
.54*** |
Social Attraction |
0.36*** |
.73*** |
0.66*** |
.83*** |
0.68*** |
.79*** |
Duration of relationship |
0.08** |
.24*** |
0.11*** |
.23*** |
0.11*** |
.12*** |
Age |
0.07* |
.11* |
-0.01 |
-.03 |
-0.03 |
-.12*** |
Gender (0 = male) |
0.04 |
-.08 |
0.04 |
-.01 |
0.09*** |
.16*** |
R square |
.68 |
|
.73 |
|
.67 |
|
Note. * p < .05; ** p < .01; *** p < .001 |
Research question RQ4b asked if there are different effects of social attraction on the quality of friendship in online, mixed-mode, and offline friendships. As Table 3 shows, the effect of social attraction on friendship quality was the highest for offline friendships (β = 0.68, p < .001), followed by mixed-mode friendships (β = 0.66, p < .000), and online friendships (β = 0.36, p < .001).
Discussion
The first aim of our study was to compare the quality of three types of friendships that are formed and/or maintained through the Internet: online, offline, and mixed-mode friendships. Earlier studies on this topic have typically compared two types of friendships that are formed and/or maintained through the Internet: online and offline friendships. In these earlier studies, online friendships were defined as friendships that originated online, while offline friendships as friendships that originated offline. However, as discussed, in our view, this dichotomous classification does not sufficiently cover contemporary friendship formation and/or maintenance. By distinguishing and comparing online, mixed-mode, and offline, we were able to make some important refinements to earlier theories and results.
In agreement with earlier studies, we found that respondents perceived offline friendships of higher quality than online friendships. However, we found that mixed-mode friendships, which are also formed online but then also migrate to offline communication modalities (i.e., telephone, face-to-face communication), were rated similar in quality as offline friendships. Thus, for bonding social capital, it is not important whether a friendship is formed online or offline, but rather, it is more important that newly formed friendships also migrate to cue-richer communication modalities, such as telephone and face-to-face contact.
We also found that the quality of all three types of friendship improved as the friendship developed over time. However, in contrast to Chan and Cheng (2004), who demonstrated that the quality of online friendships reached the level of those offline friendships within one year, in our study, the quality of online friendships remained significantly lower than that of offline friendships, even after two years. However, as discussed earlier, the differences between online and offline friendships found in Chan and Cheng’s study could have been a result of their lack of distinction between online and mixed-mode relationships. Our results suggest that although the quality of purely online friendships does differ from that of offline friendships, the quality of mixed-mode friendships was of the same level as that of offline friendships. Therefore, the important point seems to be whether people maintain the friendship online or not.
Determinants of the Quality of Online, Mixed-Mode, and Offline Friendships
The second aim of the study was to examine how the level of proximity, similarity, and social attraction differs in online, mixed-mode, and offline friendships, and to what extent these friendship determinants predict the quality of online, mixed-mode, and offline friendships.
Proximity. We asked whether the level of proximity differ in online, mixed-mode, and offline friendships. General research on offline friendships has shown that geographically close people have more opportunities to become close friends than geographically distant people (e.g., Clark & Pataki, 1995; Newcomb, 1961; Preciado et al., 2012). However, the Internet enables people to contact others regardless of their actual geographic distance. This suggests that geographic distance may be of less importance in online friendships than in offline friendships. We indeed found that offline friends lived closer to each other than mixed-mode and online friends. Suggesting that in online and mixed-mode friendships, actual geographic proximity is less important to becoming friends. This indicates that the Internet indeed globalizes friendship formation.
We also asked whether the effect of proximity on the quality of friendship would differ for online, mixed-mode, and offline friendships. We expected that frequent contact with online friends could lead to more positive feelings about these friends so that actual geographical proximity may be less important for friendship quality in online and mixed-mode friendships than in offline friendships. Previous research on offline friendships (Hays, 1985; Priest & Sawyer, 1967) found that proximity predicted the quality of offline friendships. In contrast to expectations, our results showed no effect of proximity on the quality of friendship in any of the three types of friendships. This inconsistent result may be due to the fact that all our respondents are members of social networking sites, and therefore, proximity does not matter, neither in online friendships nor in mixed-mode and offline friendships. As social networking sites are well suited to maintain friendships cheaply and easily (Donath & boyd, 2004; Ellison, Steinfield, & Lampe, 2007) we think that actual proximity is therefore of less importance than in real life.
Similarity. Theories of CMC, such as the hyperpersonal communication framework (Walther, 1996) and SIDE theory (Lea & Spears, 1992; Spears & Lea, 1992), predict that due to reduced nonverbal cues, online friends may more easily feel similar to their communication partners than offline friends. However, as discussed, social networking sites have more cues available compared to earlier text-based types of CMC. Contrary to these predictions, we found that the level of perceived similarity was lower in online friendships compared to mixed-mode and offline friendships. We did find, however, that the effect of similarity on the quality of friendship is higher for online friendships than for mixed-mode and offline friendships. These results indicate that although the level of similarity is low in online friendships, similarity is a more important determinant of friendship quality in online friendships than in the other two categories.
An explanation for the low level of similarity in online friendships is that on social networking sites, the possibility to meet someone similar is not only lower in comparison to offline settings, but also in comparison to other Internet environments. After all, on general interest social networking sites, such as Hyves and Facebook, communication is primarily organized around people, not around special topics or interests as in other Internet environments, such as chat sites and forums (boyd & Ellison, 2008). However, the lower level of similarity on social networking sites does not imply that similarity is a less important predictor of the quality of online friendships formed on social networking sites. Similarity was the most important predictor of the quality of online friendships. Our results contradict the view of some authors who claim that the Internet threatens the formation and bridging of social capital because it enables people to confine their communication to people who share precisely their interests (Putnam, 2000; VanAlstyne & Brynjolfsson, 1996). Social networking sites allow their users to meet a wide variety of similar and dissimilar communication partners. However, just like in offline settings, similarity is a core determinant of successful friendship formation and maintenance, which also enhances bonding social capital.
Social Attraction. Our results show that the level of social attraction was highest in offline and mixed-mode friendships and lowest in online friendships. We also found that the effect of social attraction was most important in offline and mixed-mode friendships and significantly less important in online friendships. A possible explanation for our findings may lie in the social part of social attraction. An important aspect of social attraction is one’s recognition that a friend or potential friend may fit within one’s existing social environment (McCroskey & McCain, 1974). When the communication between potential friends is confined to the Internet, it may be less important whether or not a potential friend fits within one’s existing social circle. Theoretically, purely online friendships can develop in isolation from real-life social circles. However, as soon as the online friendship transcends to offline settings, the question whether this friend fits within one’s existing social circle becomes more acute. This may explain why social attraction is a more important predictor of both offline and mixed-mode friendships than of online friendships.
Limitations
Although our study sheds new light on the quality of offline, mixed-mode, and of online friendships and its determinants, we recognize that our study has limitations. We used a cross sectional rather than an experimental or longitudinal design. Although we based our research design on previous theories and research, which have established a clear causal direction for several of our relationships, our results, which are based on cross-sectional data, cannot preclude the possibility of reversed causal relationships. Therefore, longitudinal research for examining causal relationships might be more suitable. Furthermore, by conducting longitudinal research, we are able to examine the changes in quality of friendships within a friendship instead of between friendships.
Implications and Future Research
Our study has several implications for future theory and research. First, our study showed that mixed-mode friendships, which originated online and migrated to offline settings, are comparable to offline friendships in several respects. Mixed-mode friendships were perceived similar in quality as were offline friendships, and the effects of the three determinants on the quality of friendship were also similar for mixed-mode and offline friendships. This implies that the crucial difference in the quality of friendships is between online friendships and mixed-mode/offline friendships. Future studies should make a clear conceptual distinction between these three types of friendships because there are major differences between online and mixed-mode friendships.
Second, this study has implications for CMC theories. To date, most CMC theories are still based on reduced-cues perspectives and the assumption of dyadic communication. Although social networking sites do not provide as many cues as in offline settings, the results of our study indicate that some of these theories, such as hyperpersonal communication framework (Walther, 1996) and SIDE theory (Lea & Spears, 1992; Spears & Lea, 1992), should be reconsidered in light of more cue-richer CMC, like social networking sites. These theories, for example, assume that, due to a lack of auditory and visual cues, CMC partners feel more similar to each other. Our study suggests that partners in online friendships do not feel more similar compared to partners in offline and mixed-mode friendships. Implying that an important assumption in earlier CMC theories must be reevaluated in light of CMC on social networking sites. Future research should pay closer attention to the differential effects of cue-rich and cue-poor CMC environments for social interaction in general, additionally relationship formation and/or maintenance in particular.
Finally, this study shows that for the quality of the friendship it does not matter how people met their friends, online versus offline, as long as the friendship also migrate to offline settings. Therefore this research also contributes to the discussion on the effects of the Internet on social capital.
Notes
1. The data used in this survey study were collected among members of the Dutch social networking site Hyves (www.hyves.nl). Hyves was at the moment of conducting the survey the most popular social networking site in the Netherlands. At that time, more than 55% (over 9 million people) of Dutch citizens were a member of Hyves. Hyves is a general interest site and is very similar to US-based social networking sites, such as Facebook. Similar to Facebook (Lampe, Ellison, & Steinfield, 2006; Joinson, 2008), and MySpace (Dwyer et al., 2007), the most important motivations for visiting Hyves is to keep in touch with existing friends and to find more information about persons met in the offline world. Because there seem to be no striking differences between Hyves and popular non-Dutch social networking sites, we believe that our findings may be generalized to users of other social networking sites.

This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.
Copyright © 2012 Cyberpsychology: Journal of Psychosocial Research on Cyberspace