Childhood risks and problematic smartphone use: Dual processes of life history strategy and psychological distress
Vol.18,No.4(2024)
Problematic smartphone use (PSU) and its negative consequences among adolescents are so commonly observed that they have become a matter of public concern. Utilizing life history (LH) theory, this study aims to examine how childhood risks (i.e., childhood harshness and unpredictability) would be associated with adolescent PSU via cognitive and emotional processes of LH strategy and psychological distress. A sample of 459 Chinese high school students aged from 14 to 18 (M = 16.90) participated in this study and voluntarily completed an anonymous survey. The results showed that childhood unpredictability and psychological distress were positively correlated with PSU, whereas childhood harshness and the two processes of LH strategy were negatively correlated with PSU. Path analysis further supported a partial mediation model, in which childhood unpredictability increased PSU, via serial mediators of the emotional process of LH strategy and psychological distress. The findings support the efficacy of LH theory in elucidating the development of behavioral addiction. However, the direct and negative association of childhood harshness with PSU was unexpected. It implicates the complexity of childhood environment on adolescent behavioral addictions and warrants further investigation.
problematic smartphone use; childhood unpredictability; life history theory; psychological distress; adolescents
Meng Xuan Zhang
Department of Medical Humanities, School of Humanities, Southeast University, Nanjing, China; Department of Psychology, Faculty of Social Sciences, University of Macau, Macao, China
Meng Xuan Zhang is an associate researcher at the School of Humanities, Southeast University, China. Her research focuses on behavioral addiction and mental health. She has conducted studies to examine the environmental factors and psychological mechanisms of problematic smartphone use, social media addiction, and gaming disorder in adolescents and young people in both cross-sectional and longitudinal studies. She also further investigates the protective roles of positive psychological factors (e.g., purpose in life and grit) as potential interventions for health problems.
Lisbeth Ku
Division of Psychology, Faculty of Health and Life Sciences, De Montfort University, Leicester, United Kingdom
Lisbeth Ku is a Senior Lecturer in Psychology at De Montfort University, UK. She has extensively published on topics including materialism, consumer behavior, and broader well-being indicators such as learning motivation, performance, body image concerns, and upcycling behavior. Recently, her research has concentrated on the influence of well-being factors on consumption patterns. Given the significant environmental challenges posed by excessive consumption, Dr. Ku collaborates with experts in design, marketing, and consumer studies to investigate and promote sustainable consumer behavior.
Anise M. S. Wu
Department of Psychology, Faculty of Social Sciences, University of Macau, Macao, China; Centre for Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Institute of Collaborative Innovation, University of Macau, Macao, China
Anise M. S. Wu is currently a professor and the associate dean (research) at the Faculty of Social Sciences, the University of Macau, and her research interest is the application of psychology to explaining, predicting, and promoting both individual and public health, with a specific focus on addictive behaviors (e.g., gambling, gaming, Internet use, smartphone use, shopping, and substance use).
Amir, D., Jordan, M. R., & Bribiescas, R. G. (2016). A longitudinal assessment of associations between adolescent environment, adversity perception, and economic status on fertility and age of menarche. PLoS One, 11(6), Article e0155883. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0155883
Belsky, J., Schlomer, G. L., & Ellis, B. J. (2012). Beyond cumulative risk: Distinguishing harshness and unpredictability as determinants of parenting and early life history strategy. Developmental Psychology, 48(3), 662–673. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024454
Belsky, J., Steinberg, L., & Draper, P. (1991). Childhood experience, interpersonal development, and reproductive strategy: An evolutionary theory of socialization. Child Development, 62(4), 647–670. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01558.x
Bertschek, I., & Niebel, T. (2016). Mobile and more productive? Firm-level evidence on the productivity effects of mobile Internet use. Telecommunications Policy, 40(9), 888–898. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.telpol.2016.05.007
Billieux, J., Maurage, P., Lopez-Fernandez, O., Kuss, D. J., & Griffiths, M. D. (2015). Can disordered mobile phone use be considered a behavioral addiction? An update on current evidence and a comprehensive model for future research. Current Addiction Reports, 2(2), 156–162. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40429-015-0054-y
Brand, M., Wegmann, E., Stark, R., Müller, A., Wölfling, K., Robbins, T. W., & Potenza, M. N. (2019). The interaction of person-affect-cognition-execution (I-PACE) model for addictive behaviors: Update, generalization to addictive behaviors beyond Internet-use disorders, and specification of the process character of addictive behaviors. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 104, 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.06.032
Buctot, D. B., Kim, N., & Kim, S. H. (2023). Comparing the mediating effect of adolescent lifestyle profiles on the relationship between smartphone addiction and health-related quality of life among male and female senior high school students in the Philippines. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 21(1), 511–528. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-021-00609-9
Casey, B. J., & Caudle, K. (2013). The teenage brain: Self-control. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(2), 82–87. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721413480170
Cha, S.-S., & Seo, B.-k. (2018). Smartphone use and smartphone addiction in middle school students in Korea: Prevalence, social networking service, and game use. Health Psychology Open, 5(1), 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1177/2055102918755046
Chang, F.-C., Chiu, C.-H., Chen, P.-H., Chiang, J.-T., Miao, N.-F., Chuang, H.-Y., & Liu, S. (2019). Children’s use of mobile devices, smartphone addiction and parental mediation in Taiwan. Computers in Human Behavior, 93, 25–32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2018.11.048
Chang, L., Lu, H. J., Lansford, J. E., Skinner, A. T., Bornstein, M. H., Steinberg, L., Dodge, K. A., Chen, B. B., Tian, Q., Bacchini, D., Deater-Deckard, K., Pastorelli, C., Alampay, L. P., Sorbring, E., Al-Hassan, S. M., Oburu, P., Malone, P. S., Di Giunta, L., Tirado, L. M. U., & Tapanya, S. (2019). Environmental harshness and unpredictability, life history, and social and academic behavior of adolescents in nine countries. Developmental Psychology, 55(4), 890–903. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000655
Chen, J. H., Su, X., Dang, L., & Wu, A. M. S. (2021). Evaluation of the psychometric properties of the Chinese Internet Gaming Disorder Checklist (C-IGDC) among Chinese adolescents. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 12, Article 721397. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.721397
China Internet Network Information Center (2021). The 48th statistical report on the development of Internet in China. http://www.cnnic.net.cn/
Copping, L. T., Campbell, A., & Muncer, S. (2013). Impulsivity, sensation seeking and reproductive behaviour: A life history perspective. Personality and Individual Differences, 54(8), 908–912. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.01.003
Daryanani, I., Hamilton, J. L., Abramson, L. Y., & Alloy, L. B. (2016). Single mother parenting and adolescent psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 44(7), 1411–1423. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-016-0128-x
Davidson, L. L., Grigorenko, E. L., Boivin, M. J., Rapa, E., & Stein, A. (2015). A focus on adolescence to reduce neurological, mental health and substance-use disability. Nature, 527(7578), S161–S166. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature16030
Del Giudice, M., & Belsky, J. (2011). The development of life history strategies: Toward a multi-stage theory. In D. M. Buss & P. H. Hawley (Eds.), The evolution of personality and individual differences (pp. 154–176). Oxford University Press.
Del Giudice, M., Gangestad, S. W., & Kaplan, H. S. (2015). Life history theory and evolutionary psychology. In David M. Buss (Ed)., The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology (pp. 1–27). John Wiley & Sons. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119125563.evpsych102
Del Giudice, M., Klimczuk, A. C. E., Traficonte, D. M., & Maestripieri, D. (2014). Autistic-like and schizotypal traits in a life history perspective: Diametrical associations with impulsivity, sensation seeking, and sociosexual behavior. Evolution and Human Behavior, 35(5), 415–424. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2014.05.007
Della Vedova, A. M., Covolo, L., Muscatelli, M., Loscalzo, Y., Giannini, M., & Gelatti, U. (2022). Psychological distress and problematic smartphone use: Two faces of the same coin? Findings from a survey on young Italian adults. Computers in Human Behavior, 132, Article 107243. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2022.107243
Doom, J. R., Vanzomeren-Dohm, A. A., & Simpson, J. A. (2016). Early unpredictability predicts increased adolescent externalizing behaviors and substance use: A life history perspective. Development and Psychopathology, 28(4pt2), 1505–1516. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579415001169
Elhai, J. D., Gallinari, E. F., Rozgonjuk, D., & Yang, H. (2020). Depression, anxiety and fear of missing out as correlates of social, non-social and problematic smartphone use. Addictive Behaviors, 105, Article 106335. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2020.106335
Ellis, B. J., Figueredo, A. J., Brumbach, B. H., & Schlomer, G. L. (2009). Fundamental dimensions of environmental risk. Human Nature, 20(2), 204–268. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-009-9063-7
Figueredo, A. J., Garcia, R. A., Menke, J. M., Jacobs, W. J., Gladden, P. R., Bianchi, J., Patch, E. A., Beck, C. J. A., Kavanagh, P. S., Sotomayor-Peterson, M., Jiang, Y., & Li, N. P. (2017). The K-SF-42: A new short form of the Arizona Life History Battery. Evolutionary Psychology, 15(1). https://doi.org/10.1177/1474704916676276
Figueredo, A. J., Vásquez, G., Brumbach, B. H., Schneider, S. M. R., Sefcek, J. A., Tal, I. R., Hill, D., Wenner, C. J., & Jacobs, W. J. (2006). Consilience and life history theory: From genes to brain to reproductive strategy. Developmental Review, 26(2), 243–275. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2006.02.002
Figueredo, A. J., Vásquez, G., Brumbach, B. H., Sefcek, J. A., Kirsner, B. R., & Jacobs, W. J. (2005). The K-factor: Individual differences in life history strategy. Personality and Individual Differences, 39(8), 1349–1360. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2005.06.009
Gardner, M. J., Thomas, H. J., & Erskine, H. E. (2019). The association between five forms of child maltreatment and depressive and anxiety disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Child Abuse & Neglect, 96, Article 104082. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2019.104082
Griskevicius, V., Tybur, J. M., Delton, A. W., & Robertson, T. E. (2011). The influence of mortality and socioeconomic status on risk and delayed rewards: A life history theory approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(6), 1015–1026. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022403
Gu, J., Guo, Y., & Wang, J.-L. (2024). Childhood maltreatment and Internet addiction among Chinese adolescents: A moderated mediation model. Journal of Affective Disorders, 350, 721–727. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2024.01.175
Hair, J. F., Black, W. C., Babin, B. J., Anderson, R. E., & Tatham, R. L. (2006). Multivariate data analysis (6th ed.). Pearson Prentice Hall.
Hardt, J., & Rutter, M. (2004). Validity of adult retrospective reports of adverse childhood experiences: Review of the evidence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(2), 260–273. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00218.x
Hazel, N. A., Hammen, C., Brennan, P. A., & Najman, J. M. (2008). Early childhood adversity and adolescent depression: The mediating role of continued stress. Psychological Medicine, 38(4), 581–589. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291708002857
Hughes, K., Bellis, M. A., Hardcastle, K. A., Sethi, D., Butchart, A., Mikton, C., Jones, L., & Dunne, M. P. (2017). The effect of multiple adverse childhood experiences on health: A systematic review and meta-analysis. The Lancet Public Health, 2(8), e356–e366. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2468-2667(17)30118-4
Hurst, J. E., & Kavanagh, P. S. (2017). Life history strategies and psychopathology: The faster the life strategies, the more symptoms of psychopathology. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38(1), 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.06.001
Kim, H. S., Vieira, J. L., Parmar, P. K., Hodgins, D. C., Shead, N. W., & Keough, M. T. (2023). Emotion dysregulation mediates the relationship between adverse childhood experiences and problematic gaming. Addictive Behaviors, 136, Article 107473. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2022.107473
Kim, S.-G., Park, J., Kim, H.-T., Pan, Z., Lee, Y., & McIntyre, R. S. (2019). The relationship between smartphone addiction and symptoms of depression, anxiety, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity in South Korean adolescents. Annals of General Psychiatry, 18, Article 1. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12991-019-0224-8
Kim, Y., Want, Y., & Oh, J. (2016). Digital media use and social engagement: How social media and smartphone use influence social activities of college students. Cyberpsychology, Behaviour, and Social Networking, 19(4), 264–269. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2015.0408
Lai, X., Huang, S., Nie, C., Yan, J. J., Li, Y., Wang, Y., & Luo, Y. (2022). Trajectory of problematic smartphone use among adolescents aged 10–18 years: The roles of childhood family environment and concurrent parent–child relationships. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 11(2), 577–587. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.2022.00047
Lee, K. E., Kim, S.-H., Ha, T.-Y., Yoo, Y.-M., Han, J.-J., Jung, J.-H., & Jang, J.-Y. (2016). Dependency on smartphone use and its association with anxiety in Korea. Public Health Reports, 131(3), 411–419. https://doi.org/10.1177/003335491613100307
Li, G., Hou, G., Yang, D., Jian, H., & Wang, W. (2019). Relationship between anxiety, depression, sex, obesity, and Internet addiction in Chinese adolescents: A short-term longitudinal study. Addictive Behaviors, 90, 421–427. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2018.12.009
Li, W., Zhang, X., Chu, M., & Li, G. (2020). The impact of adverse childhood experiences on mobile phone addiction in Chinese college students: A serial multiple mediator model. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, Article 834. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00834
Lim, M. S. M., Cheung, F. Y. L., Kho, J. M., & Tang, C. S.-K. (2020). Childhood adversity and behavioural addictions: The mediating role of emotion dysregulation and depression in an adult community sample. Addiction Research & Theory, 28(2), 116–123. https://doi.org/10.1080/16066359.2019.1594203
Lin, Y., & Liu, Q. (2020). Perceived subjective social status and smartphone addiction tendency among Chinese adolescents: A sequential mediation model. Children and Youth Services Review, 116, Article 105222. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2020.105222
Lin, Y.-H., Chang, L.-R., Lee, Y.-H., Tseng, H.-W., Kuo, T. B. J., & Chen, S.-H. (2014). Development and validation of the Smartphone Addiction Inventory (SPAI). PloS One, 9(6), Article e98312. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0098312
Lin, Y.-H., Chiang, C.-L., Lin, P.-H., Chang, L.-R., Ko, C.-H., Lee, Y.-H., & Lin, S.-H. (2016). Proposed diagnostic criteria for smartphone addiction. PLoS One, 11(11), Article e163010. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0163010
Liu, R. T. (2017). Childhood adversities and depression in adulthood: Current findings and future directions. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 24(2), 140–153. https://doi.org/10.1111/cpsp.12190
Lovibond, P. F., & Lovibond, S. H. (1995). The structure of negative emotional states: Comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33(3), 335–343. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(94)00075-U
Lu, H. J., & Chang, L. (2019). Aggression and risk‐taking as adaptive implementations of fast life history strategy. Developmental Science, 22(5), Article e12827. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12827
Lu, H. J., Liu, Y. Y., & Chang, L. (2022). Child attachment in adjusting the species-general contingency between environmental adversities and fast life history strategies. Development and Psychopathology, 34(2), 719–730. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579421001413
MacDonald, K. (1997). Life history theory and human reproductive behavior: Environmental/contextual influences and heritable variation. Human Nature, 8(4), 327–359. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02913038
Meng, H., Cao, H., Hao, R., Zhou, N., Liang, Y., Wu, L., Jiang, L., Ma, R., Li, B., Deng, L., Lin, Z., Lin, X., & Zhang, J. (2020). Smartphone use motivation and problematic smartphone use in a national representative sample of Chinese adolescents: The mediating roles of smartphone use time for various activities. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 9(1), 163–174. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.2020.00004
Mittal, C., & Griskevicius, V. (2014). Sense of control under uncertainty depends on people’s childhood environment: A life history theory approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107(4), 621–637. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037398
Orben, A., Tomova, L., & Blakemore, S.-J. (2020). The effects of social deprivation on adolescent development and mental health. The Lancet Child & Adolescent Health, 4(8), 634–640. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2352-4642(20)30186-3
Paterniti, S., Sterner, I., Caldwell, C., & Bisserbe, J.-C. (2017). Childhood neglect predicts the course of major depression in a tertiary care sample: A follow-up study. BMC Psychiatry, 17, Article 113. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-017-1270-x
Paus, T., Keshavan, M., & Giedd, J. N. (2008). Why do many psychiatric disorders emerge during adolescence? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(12), 947–957. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2513
Peñate, W., González-Loyola, M., & Oyanadel, C. (2020). The predictive role of affectivity, self-esteem and social support in depression and anxiety in children and adolescents. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(19), Article 6984. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17196984
Pera, A. (2020). The psychology of addictive smartphone behavior in young adults: Problematic use, social anxiety, and depressive stress. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 11, Article 573473. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2020.573473
Richardson, G. B., Chen, C.-C., Dai, C.-L., Hardesty, P. H., & Swoboda, C. M. (2014). Life history strategy and young adult substance use. Evolutionary Psychology, 12(5), 932–957. https://doi.org/10.1177/147470491401200506
Salas-Rodríguez, J., Gómez-Jacinto, L., & Hombrados-Mendieta, M. I. (2021). Life history theory: Evolutionary mechanisms and gender role on risk-taking behaviors in young adults. Personality and Individual Differences, 175, Article 110752. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2021.110752
Seo, J., Lee, C.-s., Lee, Y.-J., Lee, M.-S., Bhang, S.-Y., & Lee, D. (2020). The mediating effect of depressive symptoms on the relationship between adverse childhood experiences and problematic Internet use in children and adolescents. Journal of Korean Medical Science, 35(31), Article e282. https://doi.org/10.3346/jkms.2020.35.e282
Shah, P. P., & Sheth, M. S. (2018). Correlation of smartphone use addiction with text neck syndrome and SMS thumb in physiotherapy students. International Journal of Community Medicine and Public Health, 5(6), 2512–2516. https://doi.org/10.18203/2394-6040.ijcmph20182187
Squires, L. R., Hollett, K. B., Hesson, J., & Harris, N. (2021). Psychological distress, emotion dysregulation, and coping behaviour: A theoretical perspective of problematic smartphone use. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 19(4), 1284–1299. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-020-00224-0
Statista. (2019). Smartphone penetration as share of population in China 2015–2023. https://www.statista.com/statistics/321482/smartphone-user-penetration-in-china/
Statista. (2020). Number of smartphone users in China from 2015 to 2019 with a forecast until 2025. https://www.statista.com/statistics/467160/forecast-of-smartphone-users-in-china/
Sun, J., Liu, Q., & Yu, S. (2019). Child neglect, psychological abuse and smartphone addiction among Chinese adolescents: The roles of emotional intelligence and coping style. Computers in Human Behavior, 90, 74–83. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2018.08.032
Szepsenwol, O., & Simpson, J. A. (2019). Attachment within life history theory: An evolutionary perspective on individual differences in attachment. Current Opinion in Psychology, 25, 65–70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.03.005
Taouk, M. L.., Lovibond, P. F., & Laube, R. (2001). Psychometric properties of a Chinese version of the short Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS21). http://www2.psy.unsw.edu.au/dass/Chinese/ Chinese%20DASS21%20Paper.pdf
Teng, Z., Pontes, H. M., Nie, Q., Griffiths, M. D., & Guo, C. (2021). Depression and anxiety symptoms associated with Internet gaming disorder before and during the COVID-19 pandemic: A longitudinal study. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 10(1), 169–180. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.2021.00016
Tifferet, S., Agrest, S., & Benisti Shlomo, A. (2011). Problem gambling: An outcome of a life history strategy. International Gambling Studies, 11(2), 253–262. https://doi.org/10.1080/14459795.2011.599328
Vaghasiya, S., Rajpopat, N., Parmar, B., Tailor, K. A., Patel, H. V., & Varma, J. (2023). Pattern of smartphone use, prevalence and correlates of problematic use of smartphone and social media among school going adolescents. Industrial Psychiatry Journal, 32(2), 410–416. https://doi.org/10.4103/ipj.ipj_53_23
Wang, Y., Lin, Z., Hou, B., & Sun, S. (2017). The intrinsic mechanism of life history trade-offs: The mediating role of control striving. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 49(6), 783–793. https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2017.00783
Wills, T. A., Simons, J. S., Sussman, S., & Knight, R. (2016). Emotional self-control and dysregulation: A dual-process analysis of pathways to externalizing/internalizing symptomatology and positive well-being in younger adolescents. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 163, S37–S45. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2015.08.039
Wu, A. M. S., Chen, J. H., Tong, K.-K., Yu, S., & Lau, J. T. F. (2018). Prevalence and associated factors of Internet gaming disorder among community dwelling adults in Macao, China. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 7(1), 62–69. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.7.2018.12
Yang, Z., Luo, Y., Zhou, Q., Chen, F., Xu, Z., Ke, L., & Wang, Y. (2022). COVID-19-related stressors and depression in Chinese adolescents: The effects of life history strategies and gender. Journal of Affective Disorders, 304, 122–127. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2022.02.060
Yue, H., Zhang, X., Sun, J., Liu, M., Li, C., & Bao, H. (2021). The relationships between negative emotions and latent classes of smartphone addiction. PloS One, 16(3), Article e0248555. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0248555
Zhang, M. X., Hui, B. P. H., & Wu, A. M. S. (2020). Psychometric evaluation of the short form of the Arizona Life History Battery (K-SF-42): A revised Chinese version for emerging adults. Evolutionary Psychology, 18(4). https://doi.org/10.1177/1474704920969111
Zhang, M. X., & Wu, A. M. S. (2020). Effects of smartphone addiction on sleep quality among Chinese university students: The mediating role of self-regulation and bedtime procrastination. Addictive Behaviors, 111, Article 106552. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2020.106552
Zhang, M. X., Su, X., & Wu, A. M. S. (2023). Is fast life history strategy associated with poorer self-regulation and higher vulnerability to behavioral addictions? A cross-sectional study on smartphone addiction and gaming disorder. Current Psychology, 42(26), 22770–22780. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-022-03385-4
Zhang, P., Wang, J., & Guo, G. (2024). The effect of childhood socioeconomic status on youth investment risk tolerance: Based on the life history theory perspective. Personality and Individual Differences, 225, Article 112679. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2024.112679
Zhou, H., Wu, A. M. S., Su, X., Chang, L., Chen, J. H., Zhang, M. X., & Tong, K. K. (2023). Childhood environment and adulthood food addiction: Testing the multiple mediations of life history strategies and attitudes toward self. Appetite, 182, Article 106448. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2023.106448
Zhou, Y. Q., Zhang, M. X., & Guan, J. (2018). 童年经历及当前压力感知对人际信任的影响. 中国社会心理学评论 [The influence of childhood experience and current pressure perception on interpersonal trust]. Chinese Social Psychological Review, 14, 96–110.
Zhu, N., Hawk, S. T., & Chang, L. (2018). Living slow and being moral: Life history predicts the dual process of other-centered reasoning and judgments. Human Nature, 29(2), 186–209. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-018-9313-7
Zou, Y., Xia, N., Zou, Y., Chen, Z., & Wen, Y. (2019). Smartphone addiction may be associated with adolescent hypertension: A cross-sectional study among junior school students in China. BMC Pediatrics, 19, Article 310. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12887-019-1699-9
Authors’ Contribution
Meng Xuan Zhang: conceptualization, data curation, formal analysis, methodology, visualization, writing—original draft. Lisbeth Ku: data curation, writing—review & editing. Anise M. S. Wu: supervision, funding acquisition, investigation, project administration, writing—review & editing.
Editorial Record
First submission received:
April 12, 2023
Revisions received:
March 19, 2024
June 4, 2024
Accepted for publication:
June 18, 2024
Editor in charge:
Maèva Flayelle
Introduction
In the last twenty years, smartphones have become one of the most essential devices of modern life in mainland China. The number of smartphone users reached 882 million in 2019, representing a penetration rate of 53% of the population (Statista, 2019, 2020). Moreover, according to the China Internet Network Information Center (2021), 99.6% of users access the Internet via smartphones. Along with the widespread use of smartphones, a high prevalence of problematic smartphone use (PSU) was shown, particularly among adolescents. PSU, also known as smartphone addiction, generally refers to excessive, addictive, and/or inappropriate use of smartphones with adverse consequences (Billieux et al., 2015). Although PSU has not been officially classified as an addictive disorder, it shares several common symptoms with other behavioral addictive disorders such as gambling and gaming disorders: compulsive use, tolerance, withdrawal symptoms, and functional impairment (Y.-H. Lin et al., 2016). It is also consistently and positively associated with various physical and mental health problems such as musculoskeletal pain (Shah & Sheth, 2018), poor sleep quality (M. X. Zhang & Wu, 2020), and symptoms of depression and anxiety (S.-G. Kim et al., 2019).
PSU in Adolescents
Over one-fifth of adolescents exhibit addictive use of smartphones (Zou et al., 2019), with an increasing trend observed in recent years (e.g., Buctot et al., 2023; Vaghasiya et al., 2023), likely due to adolescents having weaker self-control than adults (Casey & Caudle, 2013). Given its high prevalence rate and its negative health impacts, PSU has been drawing increasing research attention in recent years.
According to the Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model (Brand et al., 2019), childhood experiences are considered biopsychological constitutions for the development of technological addictions in adolescence and adulthood. Empirical data support this claim by showing that adolescents from disadvantaged backgrounds (e.g., divorced families and lower SES) tend to exhibit a higher level of PSU (F.-C. Chang et al., 2019), while childhood neglect and psychological abuse are related to a higher risk of PSU in Chinese adolescents (Sun et al., 2019). A three-wave longitudinal study also found that early childhood SES, childhood unpredictability, and parent–child relationships had impacts on the development of adolescents’ PSU (Lai et al., 2022). While these findings shed important light on the childhood antecedents of PSU, the research effort so far is somewhat fragmented and lacks a cohesive theoretical framework that elucidates a comprehensive examination of the underlying mechanism between childhood experiences and PSU. Therefore, this study aims to fill the research gap by utilizing life history (LH) theory, a macro-level framework that describes how individuals respond to the environment and maximize fitness in the life cycle (Ellis et al., 2009; Figueredo et al., 2006).
LH Theory and PSU
LH theory asserts that individuals’ allocation of finite resources such as time and energy to various activities pertaining to growth and reproduction is related to the environments in which the individuals find themselves (Ellis et al., 2009; Figueredo et al., 2006). The childhood environment is particularly important for one’s resource allocation style and well-being in adolescence and adulthood (Belsky et al.,1991). Within LH theory, two specific facets of childhood environments may pose risks to individuals: (i) childhood harshness, defined as the morbidity-mortality rate in the early stage of one’s life, commonly assessed by childhood socioeconomic status (CSES; Ellis et al., 2009); and (ii) childhood unpredictability, referring to the extent of stability of the local environment that a child experiences (Doom et al., 2016; Ellis et al., 2009). As expected, childhood risks such as harsh parenting, physical abuse/neglect, emotional abuse/neglect, and unstable family lives (e.g., resulting from parental separation and/or divorce) have been found to be linked to mental health problems (Hughes et al., 2017; Liu, 2017). Childhood risks were also positively associated with addictive behaviors such as gambling and excessive Internet and/or social media use (Lim et al., 2020; Seo et al., 2020).
According to LH theory, the negative associations between childhood risks and addictive behaviors, including PSU, can be elucidated by one’s adoption of a specific style of resource allocation (i.e., LH strategy), determined by their childhood environment (Belsky et al.,1991; Lu & Chang, 2019). By definition, LH strategy is a specific term used in LH theory to describe how an individual allocates resources between the present and future in developmental environments (Belsky et al., 2012; Ellis et al., 2009), characterized as a continuum from “fast” (prioritizing present fitness) to “slow” (prioritizing future fitness; Ellis et al., 2009). The development of LH strategy is presumed to be evolutionarily adaptive to the environment for reproductive fitness (Hurst & Kavanagh, 2017). LH theory posits that a poorer and more unstable childhood environment results in faster LH strategy (Ellis et al., 2009). There are accumulating empirical data supporting the applicability of LH theory in explaining inter-individual differences in behavioral outcomes by considering the variances of individuals’ LH strategy resulting from phenotypic plasticity in response to environmental cues, with harsher environment priming leading to faster LH strategy (e.g., L. Chang et al., 2019; Del Giudice et al., 2014).
Previous research has clearly shown variances in psychological traits or behavioral patterns (e.g., sexual debut, age of reproduction, impulsivity, and reward preference) across cultures along the fast-slow LH continuum (e.g., L. Chang et al., 2019; MacDonald, 1997; Salas-Rodríguez et al., 2021). For instance, individuals with faster LH strategy tend to focus more on immediate rewards and be more willing to take risks for reward, whereas those with slower LH strategy tend to show opposite preferences (Amir et al., 2016; Giudice et al., 2015). LH strategy can also be manifested as cognitive and emotional processes in terms of their psychological features (Figueredo et al., 2006; Zhu et al., 2018). A cognitive process refers to impulse/cognitive control, planning, and future orientation, whereas an emotional process refers to emotional attachment and connections with others (Zhu et al., 2018). Individuals with faster LH strategy have been found to adopt higher levels of present orientation and weaker impulse and cognitive control (e.g., Copping et al., 2013; Griskevicius et al., 2011; Mittal & Griskevicius, 2014). In terms of emotional process, faster LH strategy is associated with higher likelihood of insecure attachment, poor social relationships, less social connection, and weaker support (Szepsenwol & Simpson, 2019; Zhu et al., 2018).
Despite its potential evolutionary benefits in harsh and unstable environments, faster LH strategy may not always lead to socially desirable outcomes in contemporary societies. For example, previous research has shown that individuals with faster LH strategy are more vulnerable to addictions (e.g., alcohol use disorder, food addiction, substance use, and gambling disorder; Richardson et al., 2014; Tifferet et al., 2011; H. Zhou et al., 2023). This vulnerability is plausible due to the psychosocial and/or behavioral features (e.g., preference for immediate gratification) of faster LH strategy, which increases engagement in addictive behaviors. Indeed, self-control and self-regulation are core protective traits against addiction development (Wills et al., 2016), with their foundation for further development established at a young age (Davidson et al., 2015). Although there is good evidence for the link between LH strategy and addictive behaviors, the relative influences of emotional and cognitive processes of LH strategy on one’s susceptibility to addictions deserve further investigation. This study particularly aimed to gain better knowledge of the independent roles of cognitive and emotional processes on the association between childhood risks and PSU in adolescents. Studying these dual processes can inform us not only on the role of LH strategy on PSU, but also address the relative importance of these processes in the development of addictive behaviors among adolescents who have experienced childhood risks.
The Role of Psychological Distress
The I-PACE model proposes that negative emotional responses (e.g., psychological distress), triggered by external and internal stressors, are salient psychological antecedents of addictive behaviors, including PSU (e.g., Gu et al., 2024; Ji et al., 2021; H. S. Kim et al., 2023). Previous studies have consistently demonstrated the risk effect of psychological distress on addictive Internet and smartphone use in both adults (Elhai et al., 2020; Teng et al., 2021; Wu et al., 2018) and adolescents (Chen et al., 2021; G. Li et al., 2019). As noted in the I-PACE model, technological devices such as the Internet and smartphones are major means of social interaction, entertainment, and coping with (di)stress, particularly for adolescents (Meng et al., 2020; Squires et al., 2021). Adolescence, a developmental stage characterized by rapid physical and behavioral changes, is often a time of mental and emotional turbulence that could lead to increased mental distress or disorders (Paus et al., 2008). Based on the LH framework for psychopathology (Hurst & Kavanagh, 2017), faster LH strategy would be positively related to the incidence of psychopathology, including depression and anxiety, as those with faster LH strategy are more prone to social, emotional, and psychological difficulties when growing up in unstable and adverse environments. Indeed, previous research has also shown that negative childhood environments and experiences, as well as faster LH strategy, are associated with depression and/or anxiety (Gardner et al., 2019; Yang et al., 2022). Therefore, this study would include psychological distress into the LH framework and empirically explore the potential mediating roles of the dual processes of LH strategy and psychological distress in the associations between childhood risks and PSU.
The Present Study
Based on the above theorization, this study proposed a conceptual model (Figure 1) that depicts the mediating roles of cognitive and emotional processes of LH strategy and psychological distress on the relationship between psychosocial environmental factors in childhood (i.e., childhood harshness—low CSES, and childhood unpredictability) and PSU. We hypothesized childhood harshness and childhood unpredictability to be the exogenous variables that would negatively predict not only both cognitive and emotional processes of LH strategy but also psychological distress and PSU. The cognitive and emotional processes of LH strategy would also be negatively associated with psychological distress, which in turn would be positively associated with PSU.
Figure 1. The Hypothesized Model.
Note. LH strategy: Life history strategy. The residuals of emotional and cognitive processes of LH strategy were allowed
to covary but not shown in the figure.
Methods
Participants and Procedures
Four hundred and sixty-six high school students from a public school in Hebei, China, participated in the study and voluntarily completed an anonymous survey in the Chinese language in June 2019. Seven participants, aged over 18 years old, were excluded from the final sample. Therefore, 459 students (61.9% female) aged between 14 and 18 (M = 16.90, SD = 0.62) were used in the subsequent data analyses.
Before filling out the survey, all participants were informed of their rights to anonymity, confidentiality, and withdrawal. They signed a consent form indicating their voluntary participation without any compensation. They responded to a paper-and-pencil questionnaire in their classrooms. A trained research assistant was present to offer assistance as required but otherwise did not intervene in the administration of the questionnaires. All the materials and data were kept confidential in the assistant’s laboratory. Ethics approval was obtained from the research ethics committee of the corresponding author’s institute.
Measures
PSU
It was assessed by Smartphone Addiction Inventory (SPAI) developed among Chinese youths by Y.-H. Lin et al. (2014). It included 26 items (e.g., I feel restless and irritable when the smartphone is unavailable) with a 4-point Likert response scale (1 = strongly disagree to 4 = strongly agree). A total score was computed, with a higher score indicating a higher level of PSU. The reliability of SPAI was .89 in this study.
Cognitive and Emotional Processes of LH Strategy
The two processes were measured by the K-SF-42 (Short Form of Arizona Life History Battery; K being a specific symbol in population biology originally referring to the resource allocation strategy for maximizing longevity rather than gamete production; Figueredo et al., 2005, 2017) and reflect one’s LH strategy on the “fast-slow” continuum. The scale has also been validated among Chinese students (M. X. Zhang et al., 2020).
The cognitive process was assessed by the dimension of Insight, Planning, and Control of the K-SF-42, which consists of 6 items (e.g., I can find something positive even in the worst situations; future-oriented planning; and When I encounter problems, I don’t give up until I solve them; personal control) on a 7-point Likert scale (−3 = strongly disagree to 3 = strongly agree). A higher total score on these items indicated slower LH strategy adoption, emphasizing future-oriented planning and personal control.
The emotional process was measured by three dimensions: Mother/Father Relationship Quality, Family Contact and Support, and Friend Contact and Support (Figueredo et al., 2017), totaling 18 items (e.g., How much time and attention did your biological father give you when you needed it?) on a 4-point scale (0 = not at all to 3 = a lot). A higher total score represented a higher level of adoption of slower LH strategy, emphasizing the establishment and experience of affective attachment and bonding. The reliabilities were .88 and .89, respectively, in this study.
Psychological Distress
It was measured by the Chinese version of Lovibond and Lovibond’s (1995) Depression Anxiety and Stress Scale (DASS-21; Taouk et al., 2001). The scale includes three dimensions of psychological distress, with 21 items (e.g., I found myself getting upset rather easily) on a 4-point Likert scale (0 = Do not apply to me at all to 3 = Apply to me very much). A higher total score represented a greater level of psychological distress. The Cronbach’s alpha of this scale was .93 in this study.
Childhood Harshness
It was indicated by one’s socioeconomic status in childhood (Belsky et al., 2012; Doom et al., 2016) and assessed by a 4-item scale (i.e., the childhood socioeconomic status; CSES scale) that was adopted from the 3-item measure for CSES proposed by Griskevicius et al. (2011) and validated in a Chinese sample by Wang et al., (2017). This
4-item scale has been commonly used in previous studies in Chinese populations (e.g., M. X. Zhang et al., 2020; P. Zhang et al., 2024). Participants responded to four items (e.g., My parents had a higher social status in the neighborhood when I was growing up) on a 7-point Likert response scale (1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree). Each item score was reversed and then summed up, with a higher score indicating a higher level of childhood harshness. The internal consistency of the CSES scale was .85 in this study.
Childhood Unpredictability
Referring to previous studies (M. X. Zhang et al., 2020; Y. Q. Zhou et al., 2018), a 7-item measure was used to assess childhood unpredictability. The measure requires the participants to assess the extent of their (unstable) childhood situations regarding family income, residence, and parenting. Participants answered all items on
a 5-point Likert response scale (0 = no to 4 = severe). A sample item of the measure is Parents or other members of the family often quarrel. A higher mean score indicated a higher level of unpredictability in participants’ earlier lives. The internal consistency was .68 in this study.
Background Information
Gender (0 = female, 1 = male), age (years), and length of daily smartphone use (hours) were included in this study.
Data Analyses
Preliminary statistics (i.e., descriptive statistics and correlation coefficients) were calculated in SPSS 24. The proposed path analysis model (Figure 1) was tested by AMOS 24. Following the recommendation of Hair et al. (2006), we evaluated the model fit by the following fit indices: Comparative Fit Index (CFI), Goodness of Fit Index (GFI), Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA), and Standardized Root Mean Square Residual (SRMR). Moreover, based on the bias-corrected percentile method, standardized coefficients were estimated with a 95% confidence interval by 10,000 bootstrap samples.
Results
Preliminary Analysis
All participants reported that they were smartphone users with access to smartphones. Nearly half of them (49.7%) spent over 5 hours on smartphone use every day. Table 1 shows the main variables’ means, standard deviation, and bivariate correlation coefficients. Age was not significantly associated with PSU nor with any other variables. Gender was correlated with childhood unpredictability (r = .09, p = .044) and cognitive process (r = .10, p = .026), with boys reporting higher levels of childhood unpredictability and greater adoption of the cognitive process of LH strategy.
Table 1. The Descriptive Information and Correlation Coefficients Among All the Variables (N = 459).
|
M |
SD |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
6 |
7 |
8 |
1.Problematic smartphone use |
61.48 |
10.62 |
– |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
2. Psychological distress |
15.70 |
12.24 |
.31*** |
– |
|
|
|
|
|
|
3. Cognitive process of LH strategy |
6.80 |
5.68 |
−.13** |
−.12* |
– |
|
|
|
|
|
4. Emotional process of LH strategy |
34.08 |
9.50 |
−.10* |
−.19*** |
.30*** |
– |
|
|
|
|
5. Childhood harshness |
5.03 |
1.27 |
−.13** |
−.01 |
−.08 |
−.14** |
– |
|
|
|
6. Childhood unpredictability |
0.79 |
0.64 |
.17*** |
.28*** |
.03 |
−.22*** |
.08 |
– |
|
|
7. Age |
16.90 |
0.62 |
−.02 |
.01 |
.08 |
.05 |
.02 |
.05 |
– |
|
8. Gender |
– |
– |
−.03 |
−.03 |
.10* |
−.05 |
.07 |
.09* |
−.02 |
– |
Childhood unpredictability and psychological distress had positive and significant correlations with PSU (r = .17 and .31, p < .001). Childhood harshness as well as the emotional and cognitive processes of LH strategy, were negatively associated with PSU. Moreover, both childhood harshness and unpredictability had significant and negative correlations with the emotional process of LH strategy (r = −.14, p = .002 and r = −.22, p < .001), but not with the cognitive process (p = .093 and .541).
Path Analysis
The proposed partial mediation model (Figure 1) was tested by path analysis. Gender was included as a control variable for childhood unpredictability and cognitive process given its significant correlations with them. Model fit indices reached the satisfactory levels: χ2(4) = 4.04, χ2/df = 1.01, p = .401, CFI = 1.000, GFI = .997, RMSEA = .005, 90% CI [.000, .071], SRMR= .019. All the path coefficients of the final partial model are shown in Figure 2.
Figure 2. The Final Model.
Note. *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001. LH strategy: Life history strategy. The residuals of emotional and cognitive
processes of LH strategy were allowed to covary but not shown in the figure.
Bootstrapping approach for direct and indirect effect tests showed both childhood harshness and unpredictability having significant direct but opposite effects on PSU, with childhood harshness’s effect being negative, β = −.144, 95% CI [−.239, −.042], while childhood unpredictability’s effect being positive, β = .110, 95% CI [.008, .209]. The indirect effect of childhood harshness on PSU was not significant, β = .003, 95% CI [−.033, .037], but it had a significant indirect effect on psychological distress, β = .022, 95% CI [.004, .051]. On the other hand, the indirect effect of childhood unpredictability on PSU was significant, and this was through the emotional process of LH strategy and psychological distress, and through psychological distress alone, β = .074, 95% CI [.029, .124]. Moreover, cognitive and emotional processes of LH strategy also exerted negative indirect effects through psychological distress on PSU, β = −.026, 95% CI [−.056, −.003] and β = −.028, 95% CI [−.067, −.001] respectively. The summary of the coefficients of both direct and indirect effects in the final partial model explains 13.5% of the variance in PSU.
Discussion
Smartphones have gradually become an essential part of daily life for Chinese adolescents (Cha & Seo, 2018). Our findings showed that many of our Chinese adolescent participants used smartphones for long hours (i.e., five hours or above) every day. Based on LH theory, the present study proposed and tested a conceptual model that denoted the effects of childhood environment risk factors (i.e., high childhood harshness and unpredictability) on PSU via dual processes of LH strategy (i.e., emotional and cognitive processes) and psychological distress. Results of path analysis showed that this conceptual model generally fitted the data well. Childhood harshness and unpredictability were significantly and negatively related to the emotional, but not cognitive, process of LH strategy. The emotional process of LH strategy was negatively related to psychological distress, which was positively associated with PSU.
Based on LH theory, LH strategy explains how the cues of environmental risks lead to individuals showing distinct psychological features in emotional and cognitive processes that prompt risk-taking behaviors, antisocial behaviors, or even addictive behaviors (Del Giudice & Belsky, 2011; M. X. Zhang et al., 2023; Zhu et al., 2018). Our findings lend support to the application of LH theory to PSU by demonstrating that self-reported harshness and unpredictability in the childhood environment harm the emotional process of LH strategy, which then promotes both psychological distress and PSU in this adolescent sample. Previous studies have shown that individuals with slower LH strategy would develop stronger emotional bonds and social connections with others, whereas individuals with faster LH strategy tend to exhibit weaker bonds and connections (Zhu et al., 2018). These patterns could be explained by a more secure attachment style among those who grew up in a stable and abundant environment (Lu et al., 2022). Moreover, consistent with previous research that showed that individuals lacking social support or connections tend to suffer in their mental health (e.g., Orben et al., 2020; Qi et al., 2020), our findings reflected a negative effect of the emotional process of LH strategy on psychological distress and PSU in adolescents. Although our study showed a relatively weak or even non-significant relationship between childhood risk factors and the cognitive process of LH strategy, the cognitive process also exerted negative effects on psychological distress and PSU. In the previous studies, consistent results suggested environmental factors, especially for childhood risks, influence the development of fast/slow LH strategy (Ellis et al., 2009), which was regarded as a whole psychological structure. As many features exist in LH strategy, including psychological ones, our findings further reflect that childhood risks seem to have more effects on psychological features related to the emotional process of LH strategy.
The relationship between LH strategy and PSU that our conceptual model hypothesized and our data confirmed further supports the application of LH theory to behavioral addiction. Specifically, the cognitive process of LH strategy had both negative direct and indirect (via psychological distress) effects on PSU. These results add to an increasing amount of empirical evidence that suggests that a cluster of related traits in LH dimensions (short vs. long time perspectives, immediate vs. delayed rewards, and high vs. low levels of risk-taking) tend to exert opposite effects on individuals’ mental and behavioral health (Figueredo et al., 2006). Furthermore, the emotional process of LH strategy only exerted an indirect effect via psychological distress on adolescents’ PSU. Lower emotional attachment and less social support would trigger more depression or anxiety (Peñate et al., 2020), which further contributes to adolescents’ PSU. While further replication is needed, the finding suggests addictive behaviors such as problematic smartphone and/or Internet use may stem from some types of fitness-enhancing functions. From the LH perspective, immersing in behaviors that lead to immediate rewards may be more rational than engaging in activities that require long-term investment if one’s environment is outside of one’s control. As constructive use of smartphones and digital media can provide immediate and positive outcomes ranging from enhanced productivity (Bertschek & Niebel, 2016) to facilitated social engagement (Y. Kim et al., 2016), it is not difficult to see why smartphone use could start as “adaptive” behavior that contributes to adolescents’ attempt to control their environment.
Consistent with previous studies (e.g., Della Vedova et al., 2022; Yue et al., 2021), our study showed psychological distress as a significant correlate of PSU. According to the pathway model of PSU (Billieuex et al., 2015), PSU may stem from the desire to obtain reassurance and to cope with emotional vulnerability and distress. Therefore, when people are experiencing a higher level of depression, anxiety, and/or stress, they may become vulnerable to PSU. More importantly, psychological distress was found to mediate the effects of emotional and cognitive processes of slow LH strategy on PSU. Furthermore, the effect of childhood unpredictability on PSU was also mediated by psychological distress. Previous research consistently demonstrated the impact of childhood adversity on psychological distress (e.g., anxiety and depression; Daryanani et al., 2016; Hazel et al., 2008), a salient predictor of PSU (e.g., Lee et al., 2016; Pera, 2020). While our finding is coherent with previous findings on other mental disorders like depression and anxiety (Hurst & Kavanagh, 2017), it also extends the explanation of the effects of LH strategy on both emotional and cognitive processes on adolescents’ mental health. For adolescents, it seems psychological distress does not only impair their mental health, but it is also one of the underlying mechanisms that link environmental factors and LH strategy to PSU.
Apart from indirect effects via LH strategy and psychological distress, childhood unpredictability also had a direct positive effect on PSU, suggesting that those who went through unstable and adverse childhood experiences (e.g., parental separation and/or divorce, emotional and/or physical neglect/ abuse) were more likely to develop behavioral addiction in adolescence. As proposed by the I-PACE model, childhood experiences serve as biopsychological constitution factors and thus salient determinants of one’s vulnerability to behavioral addictions such as problematic Internet-related use. Our finding is also consistent with the risk-enhancing effect of adverse childhood experiences (e.g., Physical/emotional neglect or abuse) on PSU reported in previous research (e.g., W. Li et al., 2020; Lim et al., 2020). Although the current study adds to the existing literature by elucidating two mechanisms (i.e., via LH strategy formation and increased susceptibility to psychological distress) underlying the link between an unstable childhood environment and adolescent PSU, other underlying biopsychosocial mechanisms may also exist and deserve further investigation. Moreover, it is important to note that there was a negative direct effect of childhood harshness (measured by CSES in the current study). This suggests that adolescents from advantaged families (demonstrated by high SES in childhood) are more likely to exhibit problematic smartphone use. This finding can be plausibly attributed to the multiple pathways through which CSES influences PSU. For example, Y. Lin and Liu (2020) found a positive indirect effect of SES on PSU via higher levels of online psychological need satisfaction but a negative indirect effect via relative deprivation. So, Chinese adolescents growing up from advantaged families may have more access to smartphones or have more opportunities to own a smartphone, which allows them to spend more time on its functions.
Some limitations of this study, along with their implications for future research directions, should be noted. First, convenience sampling may limit the generalization of findings to the entire adolescent population. The current findings should be further tested in a more representative sample of adolescents, encompassing a wider age range and cultural diversity. Second, our cross-sectional survey design precluded the examination of prospective or causal effects of external environments and internal mechanisms on PSU. Longitudinal or experimental research is warranted to verify the replicability of the findings regarding the proposed mediations. Moreover, the assessment tools may introduce self-reported biases. Objective data, especially for PSU, could be collected using advanced technology (e.g., mobile tracking applications for smartphone usage) in future studies. Additionally, the measurement of childhood unpredictability relied on retrospective data of early life events, which might be susceptible to recall bias and suffer from lower reliability and validity (Hardt & Rutter, 2004; Paterniti et al., 2017). Evaluating one’s childhood experience through reports from significant others, such as parents, could be considered in future studies to address this issue. Given the unexpected association between CSES and PSU observed, future studies may not only further examine potential underlying mechanisms, but also consider using other indicators for childhood harshness to ascertain whether similar relationships are observed. Based on the current path model of PSU, studies could expand it to include other psychological potential mediators (e.g., motivations of use) and/or extend it to other addictive behaviors for a more comprehensive understanding of pathways for addiction development in contemporary societies. This would aid in designing cost-effective interventions.
Conflict of Interest
The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.
Acknowledgement
The research was supported by the University of Macau (Ref #: MYRG2019-00014-FSS) and the Specialized Subsidy Scheme for Macao Higher Education Institutions in the Area of Research in Humanities and Social Sciences (21/DSESHSS-UM/2019). The funding source had no role in the study design, collection, analysis or interpretation of the data, writing the manuscript, or the decision to submit the paper for publication.
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike 4.0 International License.
Copyright © 2024 Meng Xuan Zhang, Lisbeth Ku, Anise M. S. Wu