Gaming as a coping strategy during the COVID-19 pandemic

Vol.17,No.3(2023)

Abstract

Early in the COVID-19 pandemic, social interactions were constrained by physical distancing guidelines. Consequently, some individuals may have turned to video games to cope with isolation and negative emotions. Previous studies have shown that people who struggle with anxiety and depression are at particular risk for developing problem gaming behaviours. However, there is a paucity of longitudinal research testing pathways from negative emotionality to problem gaming behaviours, especially during the COVID-19 pandemic. Accordingly, we conducted a multi-wave longitudinal study and predicted that high levels of emotional vulnerability (anxiety and depression) in the first month of the pandemic would prospectively relate to elevated time spent gaming and related problems six months later. We also predicted that elevated coping motives for gaming would mediate these associations. A sample of 332 Canadian gamers (Mage = 33.79; 60.8% men) completed three surveys on Prolific, with the first occurring in April 2020 (one-month after the declared COVID-19 state of emergency) and subsequent surveys were spaced three months apart. High initial levels of emotional vulnerability predicted excessive time spent gaming, as well as related problems, six months into the pandemic. Elevated coping motives for gaming uniquely mediated these pathways. This longitudinal study is the first to show that negative emotionality was a vulnerability factor for coping-related problem gaming during the COVID-19 pandemic. As we continue to cope with the longer-lasting impacts of the pandemic, it will be important for individuals who struggle with mood and anxiety issues to find more effective ways of coping.


Keywords:
gaming; coping; COVID-19; pandemic; problematic gaming; anxiety; depression
Author biographies

Rebecca E. Lewinson

Department of Psychology, Faculty of Health, York University, Toronto, Canada

Rebecca Lewinson is a PhD candidate in Clinical Psychology at York University. Her research interests focus on health psychology, including pain inferences, numerical anchoring, and problematic gaming.

Jeffrey D. Wardell

Department of Psychology, Faculty of Health, York University, Toronto, Canada; Institute for Mental Health Policy Research, Centre for Addition and Mental Health, Toronto, Canada; Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada

Dr. Jeffrey D. Wardell is an Assistant Professor of Psychology at York University and a registered Clinical Psychologist with expertise in the assessment and treatment of addictive behaviour. His research seeks to elucidate causes of addictive behaviour, with a particular focus on young adults, people living with HIV, and people who use cannabis for therapeutic purposes.

Naama Kronstein

Department of Psychology, Faculty of Health, York University, Toronto, Canada

Naama Kronstein is a Master’s student in Clinical Psychology at York University. Her research interests include addictions and behavioural addictions (i.e., gaming disorder), cognition, and neuropsychology. Her research focuses on individual differences as either protective or risk factors (i.e., personality) during early adulthood.

Karli K. Rapinda

Department of Psychology, Faculty of Arts, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, Canada

Karli K. Rapinda is a PhD candidate in Clinical Psychology at the University of Manitoba. Her research interests include addiction and addiction-related stigma. She is a Vanier scholar who researches addiction and addiction-related stigma. She has collaborated both nationally and internationally in randomized controlled trial research. 

Tyler Kempe

Department of Psychology, Faculty of Arts, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, Canada

Tyler Kempe is a PhD student in Clinical Psychology at the University of Manitoba. His research interests include addictive substances (i.e., tobacco, alcohol, cannabis) and behaviours (i.e., gaming disorder, gambling), anxiety and depression, and technology-delivered treatment interventions. 

Joel D. Katz

Department of Psychology, Faculty of Health, York University, Toronto, Canada

Dr. Joel Katz is a Distinguished Research Professor of Psychology and Tier 1 Canada Research Chair in Health Psychology at York University. He is the Research Director of the Pain Research Unit and Lead Researcher of the Transitional Pain Service both in the Department of Anesthesia and Pain Management at the Toronto General Hospital in addition to serving as a Professor in the Department of Anesthesiology & Pain Medicine at the University of Toronto. Dr. Katz’s research is aimed, broadly, at understanding the psychological, emotional, and biomedical factors involved in acute and chronic pain.

Hyoun S. Kim

Department of Psychology, Toronto Metropolitan University, Toronto, Canada

Dr. Hyoun S. Kim is an Assistant Professor in the Department of Psychology at Toronto Metropolitan University and an Adjunct Scientist at University of Ottawa Institute of Mental Health Research at The Royal. Dr. Kim is also the Chair of CPA’s Addiction Psychology, and directs the Addictions and Mental Health (ADMH) Laboratory. Dr. Kim’s clinical interests are in providing evidence-based care for people with co-occurring addictions and mental health difficulties. Relatedly, his research interest lies in developing an integrated treatment for substance and behavioural addictions and their mental health comorbidities. 

Matthew T. Keough

Department of Psychology, Faculty of Health, York University, Toronto, Canada

Dr. Matthew Keough is an Associate Professor in the Department of Psychology. He is a registered clinical psychologist and former Chair of the Addiction Psychology section of the Canadian Psychological Association. He was previously an Assistant Professor in the Department of Psychology at the University of Manitoba (2017 – 2019). Dr. Keough’s research focuses on improving understanding of the etiology and treatment of addictive behaviour, including both substance use and behavioural addictions (e.g., problem gambling). Dr. Keough’s work is mechanism-focused and is rooted in motivational models of personality and cognitive theory. 

References

Agrawal, S., Sobell, M. B., & Sobell, L. C. (2008). The timeline followback: A scientifically and clinically useful tool for assessing substance use. In R. F. Belli, F. P. Stafford, & D. F. Alwin (Eds.), Calendar and time diary methods in life course research (pp. 57–68). Sage. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781412990295

Alyami, H. S., Naser, A. Y., Dahmash, E. Z., Alyami, M. H., & Alyami, M. S. (2021). Depression and anxiety during the COVID-19 pandemic in Saudi Arabia: A cross-sectional study. International Journal of Clinical Practice, 75(7), Article e14244. https://doi.org/10.1111/ijcp.14244

APA. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). American Psychiatric Association.

Amin, K. P., Griffiths, M. D., & Dsouza, D. D. (2022). Online gaming during the COVID-19 pandemic in India: Strategies for work-life balance. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 20(1), 296–302. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-020-00358-1

Balhara, Y. P. S., Garg, H., Kumar, S., & Bhargava, R. (2018). Gaming disorder as a consequence of attempt at self- medication: Empirical support to the hypothesis. Asian Journal of Psychiatry, 31, 98–99. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajp.2018.02.013

Ballabio, M., Griffiths, M. D., Urbán, R., Quartiroli, A., Demetrovics, Z., & Király, O. (2017). Do gaming motives mediate between psychiatric symptoms and problematic gaming? An empirical survey study. Addiction Research & Theory, 25(5), 397–408. https://doi.org/10.1080/16066359.2017.1305360

Baptist Mohseni, N., Morris, V., Vedelago, L., Kempe, T., Rapinda, K., Mesmer, E., Bilevicius, E., Wardell, J. D., MacKillop, J., & Keough, M. T. (2022). A longitudinal approach to understanding risk factors for problem alcohol use during the COVID-19 pandemic. Alcohol: Clinical and Experimental Research, 46(3), 434–446. https://doi.org/10.1111/acer.14774

Barnett, E., Sussman, S., Smith, C., Rohrbach, L. A., & Spruijt-Metz, D. (2012). Motivational interviewing for adolescent substance use: A review of the literature. Addictive Behaviors, 37(12), 1325–1334. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2012.07.001

Barr, M., & Copeland-Stewart, A. (2022). Playing video games during the COVID-19 pandemic and effects on players’ well-being. Games and Culture, 17(1), 122–139. https://doi.org/10.1177/15554120211017036

Brown, T. A., & Barlow, D. H. (2009). A proposal for a dimensional classification system based on the shared features of the DSM-IV anxiety and mood disorders: Implications for assessment and treatment. Psychological Assessment, 21(3), 256–271. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016608

Calvano, C., Engelke, L., Di Bella, J., Kindermann, J., Renneberg, B., & Winter, S. M. (2022). Families in the COVID-19 pandemic: Parental stress, parent mental health and the occurrence of adverse childhood experiences—results of a representative survey in Germany. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 31(7), 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-021-01739-0

Cameron-Blake, E., Breton, C., Sim, P., Tatlow, H., Hale, T., Wood, A., Smith, J., Sawatsky, J., Parsons, Z., & Tyson, K. (2021). Variation in the Canadian provincial and territorial responses to COVID-19 (Blavatnik School of Government Working Paper Series No. 039). https://www.bsg.ox.ac.uk/sites/default/files/2021-03/BSG-WP-2021-039.pdf

Caro, C., & Popovac, M. (2021). Gaming when things get tough? Examining how emotion regulation and coping self-efficacy influence gaming during difficult life situations. Games and Culture, 16(5), 611–631. https://doi.org/10.1177/1555412020944622

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2020). Anxiety and Depression Household Pulse Survey. https://www.cdc.gov/nchs/covid19/pulse/mental-health.htm

Chantal, Y., & Vallerand, R. J. (1996). Skill versus luck: A motivational analysis of gambling involvement. Journal of Gambling Studies, 12(4), 407–418. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01539185

Cheung, G. W., & Rensvold, R. B. (2002). Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 9(2), 233–255. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15328007SEM0902_5

Cooper, M. L. (1994). Motivations for alcohol use among adolescents: Development and validation of a four-factor model. Psychological Assessment, 6(2), 117–128. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.6.2.117

Cooper, M. L., Frone, M. R., Russell, M., & Mudar, P. (1995). Drinking to regulate positive and negative emotions: A motivational model of alcohol use. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(5), 990–1005. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.69.5.990

Cooper, M. L., Kuntsche, E., Levitt, A., Barber, L. L., & Wolf, S. (2016). Motivational models of substance use: A review of theory and research on motives for using alcohol, marijuana, and tobacco. In K. J. Sher (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of substance use and substance use disorders (pp. 375–421). Oxford University Press.

Cox, W. M., & Klinger, E. (1988). A motivational model of alcohol use. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97(2), 168–180. https://doi.org/10.1037//0021-843x.97.2.168

Craske, M. G. (2012). Transdiagnostic treatment for anxiety and depression. Depression and Anxiety, 29(9), 749–753. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.21992

Doi, S., Ito, M., Takebayashi, Y., Muramatsu, K., & Horikoshi, M. (2018). Factorial validity and invariance of the Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ)-9 among clinical and non-clinical populations. PloS One, 13(7), Article e0199235. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199235

Dozois, D. J. A. (2021). Anxiety and depression in Canada during the COVID-19 pandemic: A national survey. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie canadienne, 62(1), 136–142. https://doi.org/10.1037/cap0000251

Enders, C. K. (2010). Applied missing data analysis. Guilford Press.

Entertainment Software Association of Canada. (2018). Essential Facts about the Candian Game Industry 2018. https://theesa.ca/resource/essential-facts-2018/

Entertainment Software Association of Canada. (2020). Real Canadian Gamer Essential Facts 2020. https://essentialfacts2020.ca/

Fazeli, S., Zeidi, I. M., Lin, C.-Y., Namdar, P., Griffiths, M. D., Ahorsu, D. K., & Pakpour, A. H. (2020). Depression, anxiety, and stress mediate the associations between internet gaming disorder, insomnia, and quality of life during the COVID-19 outbreak. Addictive Behaviors Reports, 12, Article 100307. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.abrep.2020.100307

Fountoulakis, K. N., Apostolidou, M. K., Atsiova, M. B., Filippidou, A. K., Florou, A. K., Gousiou, D. S., Katsara, A. R., Mantzari, S. N., Padouva-Markoulaki, M., Papatriantafyllou, E. I., Sacharidi, P. I., Tonia, A. I., Tsagalidou, E. G., Zymara, V. P., Prezerakos, P. E., Koupidis, S. A., Fountoulakis, N. K., & Chrousos, G. P. (2021). Self-reported changes in anxiety, depression and suicidality during the COVID-19 lockdown in Greece. Journal of Affective Disorders, 279, 624–629. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2020.10.061

Fritz, M. S., & MacKinnon, D. P. (2007). Required sample size to detect the mediated effect. Psychological Science, 18(3), 233–239. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01882.x

Fu, W., Wang, C., Zou, L., Guo, Y., Lu, Z., Yan, S., & Mao, J. (2020). Psychological health, sleep quality, and coping styles to stress facing the COVID-19 in Wuhan, China. Translational Psychiatry, 10(1), Article 225. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-020-00913-3

Giardina, A., Di Blasi, M., Schimmenti, A., King, D. L., Starcevic, V., & Billieux, J. (2021). Online gaming and prolonged self-isolation: Evidence from Italian gamers during the COVID-19 outbreak. Clin Neuropsychiatry, 18(1), 65–74. https://doi.org/10.36131/cnfioritieditore20210106

Gillen, P., Neill, R. D., Manthorpe, J., Mallett, J., Schroder, H., Nicholl, P., Currie, D., Moriarty, J., Ravalier, J., McGrory, S., & McFadden, P. (2022). Decreasing wellbeing and increasing use of negative coping strategies: The effect of the COVID-19 pandemic on the UK health and social care workforce. Epidemiologia, 3(1), 26–39. https://doi.org/10.3390/epidemiologia3010003

González-Bueso, V., Santamaría, J. J., Fernández, D., Merino, L., Montero, E., & Ribas, J. (2018). Association between internet gaming disorder or pathological video-game use and comorbid psychopathology: A comprehensive review. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 15(4), Article 668. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph15040668

Gorman, J. M. (1996). Comorbid depression and anxiety spectrum disorders. Depression and Anxiety, 4(4), 160–168. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1520-6394(1996)4:4<160::AID-DA2>3.0.CO;2-J

Hellström, C., Nilsson, K. W., Leppert, J., & Åslund, C. (2015). Effects of adolescent online gaming time and motives on depressive, musculoskeletal, and psychosomatic symptoms. Upsala Journal of Medical Sciences, 120(4), 263–275. https://doi.org/10.3109/03009734.2015.1049724

Hirschfeld, R. M. A. (2001). The comorbidity of major depression and anxiety disorders: Recognition and management in primary care. Primary Care Companion Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 3(6), 244–254. https://doi.org/10.4088/pcc.v03n0609

Hu, L.-t., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 1–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118

Hyland, P., Shevlin, M., McBride, O., Murphy, J., Karatzias, T., Bentall, R. P., Martinez, A., & Vallieres, F. (2020). Anxiety and depression in the Republic of Ireland during the COVID-19 pandemic. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 142(3), 249–256. https://doi.org/10.1111/acps.13219

Jain, A., & Jolly, T. S. (2021). Omicron (B.1.1.529) COVID-19 variant: A mental health perspective on lessons learned and future challenges. Primary Care Companion for CNS Disorders, 23(6), Article 38797. https://doi.org/10.4088/PCC.21com03206

Jo, Y. S., Bhang, S. Y., Choi, J. S., Lee, H. K., Lee, S. Y., & Kweon, Y.-S. (2019). Clinical characteristics of diagnosis for internet gaming disorder: Comparison of DSM-5 IGD and ICD-11 GD diagnosis. Journal of Clinical Medicine, 8(7), Article 945. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm8070945

Kim, N. R., Hwang, S. S.-H., Choi, J.-S., Kim, D.-J., Demetrovics, Z., Kiraly, O., Nagygyorgy, K., Griffiths, M. D., Hyun, S. Y., Youn, H. C., & Choi, S.-W. (2016). Characteristics and psychiatric symptoms of internet gaming disorder among adults using self-reported DSM-5 criteria. Psychiatry Investigation, 13(1), 58–66. https://doi.org/10.4306/pi.2016.13.1.58

King, D. L., & Delfabbro, P. H. (2016). The cognitive psychopathology of internet gaming disorder in adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 44(8), 1635–1645. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-016-0135-y

King, D. L., Delfabbro, P. H., Billieux, J., & Potenza, M. N. (2020). Problematic online gaming and the COVID-19 pandemic. Journal of Behavioral Addiction, 9(2), 184–186. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.2020.00016

King, D. L., Delfabbro, P. H., Perales, J. C., Deleuze, J., Kiraly, O., Krossbakken, E., & Billieux, J. (2019). Maladaptive player-game relationships in problematic gaming and gaming disorder: A systematic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 73, Article 101777. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2019.101777

Király, O., Urbán, R., Griffiths, M. D., Ágoston, C., Nagygyörgy, K., Kökönyei, G., & Demetrovics, Z. (2015). The mediating effect of gaming motivation between psychiatric symptoms and problematic online gaming: An online survey. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 17(4), Article e3515. https://doi.org/10.2196/jmir.3515

Kline, R. B. (2010). Principles and practice of Structural Equation Modeling. The Guilford Press.

Kline, R. B. (2013). Beyond significance testing: Statistics reform in the behavioral sciences. American Psychological Association.

Krause, K. H., Verlenden, J. V., Szucs, L. E., Swedo, E. A., Merlo, C. L., Niolon, P. H., Leroy, Z. C., Sims, V. M., Deng, X., Lee, S., Rasberry, C. N., & Underwood, J. M. (2022). Disruptions to school and home life among high school students during the COVID-19 pandemic—Adolescent behaviors and experiences survey, United States, January–June 2021. MMWR supplements, 71(3), Article 28. https://doi.org/10.15585/mmwr.su7103a5

Kroenke, K., & Spitzer, R. L. (2002). The PHQ-9: A new depression diagnostic and severity measure. Psychiatric Annals, 32(9), 509–515. https://doi.org/10.3928/0048-5713-20020901-06

Kroenke, K., Spitzer, R. L., & Williams, J. B. W. (2001). The PHQ-9: Validity of a brief depression severity measure. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 16(9), 606–613. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1525-1497.2001.016009606.x

Kuss, D. J., Griffiths, M. D., & Pontes, H. M. (2017). Chaos and confusion in DSM-5 diagnosis of internet gaming disorder: Issues, concerns, and recommendations for clarity in the field. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 6(2), 103–109. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.5.2016.062

Kuss, D. J., Louws, J., & Wiers, R. W. (2012). Online gaming addiction? Motives predict addictive play behavior in massively multiplayer online role-playing games. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 15(9), 480–485. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2012.0034

Lange, B. P., & Schwab, F. (2018). Game on: Sex differences in the production and consumption of video games. In J. Breuer, D. Pietschmann, B. Liebold, & B. P. Lange (Eds.), Evolutionary psychology and digital games (pp. 193–204). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315160825

Lemmens, J. S., Valkenburg, P. M., & Gentile, D. A. (2015). The Internet Gaming Disorder Scale. Psychological Assessment, 27(2), 567–582. https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000062

Liu, L., Yao, Y.-W., Li, C.-s. R., Zhang, J.-T., Xia, C.-C., Lan, J., Ma, S.-S., Zhou, N., & Fang, X.-Y. (2018). The comorbidity between internet gaming disorder and depression: Interrelationship and neural mechanisms. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 9, Article 154. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00154

López-Cabarcos, M. Á., Ribeiro-Soriano, D., & Piñeiro-Chousa, J. (2020). All that glitters is not gold. The rise of gaming in the COVID-19 pandemic. Journal of Innovation & Knowledge, 5(4), 289–296. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jik.2020.10.004

Löwe, B., Decker, O., Müller, S., Brähler, E., Schellberg, D., Herzog, W., & Herzberg, P. Y. (2008). Validation and standardization of the Generalized Anxiety Disorder Screener (GAD-7) in the general population. Medical Care, 46(3), 266–274. https://doi.org/10.1097/MLR.0b013e318160d093

Lucas, K., & Sherry, J. L. (2004). Sex differences in video game play: A communication-based explanation. Communication Research, 31(5), 499–523. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650204267930

Männikkö, N., Ruotsalainen, H., Miettunen, J., Pontes, H. M., & Kääriäinen, M. (2020). Problematic gaming behaviour and health-related outcomes: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Health Psychology, 25(1), 67–81. https://doi.org/10.1177/1359105317740414

Marino, C., Canale, N., Vieno, A., Caselli, G., Scacchi, L., & Spada, M. M. (2020). Social anxiety and internet gaming disorder: The role of motives and metacognitions. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 9(3), 617–628. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.2020.00044

Marraudino, M., Bonaldo, B., Vitiello, B., Bergui, G. C., & Panzica, G. (2022). Sexual differences in internet gaming disorder (IGD): From psychological features to neuroanatomical networks. Journal of Clinical Medicine, 11(4), Article 1018. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm11041018

McCormack, C. (2021). Differences in the economic impacts of COVID-19 across the provinces and territories. Statistics Canada. https://doi.org/10.25318/36280001202100600001-eng

McMahon, G., Douglas, A., Casey, K., & Ahern, E. (2022). Disruption to well-being activities and depressive symptoms during the COVID-19 pandemic: The mediational role of social connectedness and rumination. Journal of Affective Disorders, 309, 274–281. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2022.04.142

Melodia, F., Canale, N., & Griffiths, M. D. (2020). The role of avoidance coping and escape motives in problematic online gaming: A systematic literature review. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 20, 996–1022. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-020-00422-w

Miller, W. R., Toscova, R. T., Miller, J. H., & Sanchez, V. (2000). A theory-based motivational approach for reducing alcohol/drug problems in college. Health Education & Behavior, 27(6), 744–759. https://doi.org/10.1177/109019810002700609

Mohanty, J., Chokkanathan, S., & Alberton, A. M. (2022). COVID‐19–related stressors, family functioning and mental health in Canada: Test of indirect effects. Family Relations, 71(2), 445–462. https://doi.org/10.1111/fare.12635

Moitra, E., Christopher, P. P., Anderson, B. J., & Stein, M. D. (2015). Coping-motivated marijuana use correlates with DSM-5 cannabis use disorder and psychological distress among emerging adults. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 29(3), 627–632. https://doi.org/10.1037/adb0000083

Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. (2012). 1998-2012. Mplus User’s Guide (7th ed.). Muthén & Muthén.

Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2017). Mplus user’s guide (Vol. 8). Muthén & Muthén.

Myrseth, H., Notelaers, G., Strand, L. A., Borud, E. K., & Olsen, O. K. (2017). Introduction of a new instrument to measure motivation for gaming: The electronic gaming motives questionnaire. Addiction, 112(9), 1658–1668. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.13874

Na, E., Lee, H., Choi, I., & Kim, D.-J. (2017). Comorbidity of internet gaming disorder and alcohol use disorder: A focus on clinical characteristics and gaming patterns. The American Journal on Addictions, 26(4), 326–334. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajad.12528

Palan, S., & Schitter, C. (2018). Prolific.ac—A subject pool for online experiments. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Finance, 17, 22–27. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbef.2017.12.004

Pallavicini, F., Pepe, A., & Mantovani, F. (2022). The effects of playing video games on stress, anxiety, depression, loneliness, and gaming disorder during the early stages of the COVID-19 pandemic: PRISMA systematic review. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 25(6), 334–354. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2021.0252

Panteli, M., Papantoniou, A., Vaiouli, P., Leonidou, C., & Panayiotou, G. (2022). Feeling down in lockdown: Effects of COVID-19 pandemic on emotionally vulnerable individuals. The Counseling Psychologist, 50(3), 335–358. https://doi.org/10.1177/00110000211064905

Peter, S. C., Ginley, M. K., & Pfund, R. A. (2020). Assessment and treatment of internet gaming disorder. Journal of Health Service Psychology, 46(1), 29–36. https://doi.org/10.1007/s42843-020-00005-2

Petry, N. M., & O’Brien, C. P. (2013). Internet gaming disorder and the DSM-5. Addiction, 108(7), 1186–1187. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.12162

Plett, D., Pechlivanoglou, P., & Coyte, P. C. (2022). The impact of provincial lockdown policies and COVID-19 case and mortality rates on anxiety in Canada. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 76(9), 468–474. https://doi.org/10.1111/pcn.13437

Pollack, M. H. (2005). Comorbid anxiety and depression. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 66(Suppl 8), 22–29. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16336033

Pontes, H. M., & Griffiths, M. D. (2015). Measuring DSM-5 internet gaming disorder: Development and validation of a short psychometric scale. Computers in Human Behavior, 45, 137–143. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.12.006

Prolific. (2018). Using attention checks as a measure of data quality. https://researcher-help.prolific.co/hc/en-gb/articles/360009223553-Using-attention-checks-as-a-measure-of-data-quality

Przybylski, A. K., & Weinstein, N. (2019). Investigating the motivational and psychosocial dynamics of dysregulated gaming: Evidence from a preregistered cohort study. Clinical Psychological Science, 7(6), 1257–1265. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702619859341

Przybylski, A. K., Weinstein, N., & Murayama, K. (2017). Internet gaming disorder: Investigating the clinical relevance of a new phenomenon. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 174(3), 230–236. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.16020224

Rehm, J., Kilian, C., Ferreira-Borges, C., Jernigan, D., Monteiro, M., Parry, C. D. H., Sanchez, Z. M., & Manthey, J. (2020). Alcohol use in times of the COVID 19: Implications for monitoring and policy. Drug and Alcohol Review, 39(4), 301–304. https://doi.org/10.1111/dar.13074

Restubog, S. L. D., Ocampo, A. C. G., & Wang, L. (2020). Taking control amidst the chaos: Emotion regulation during the COVID-19 pandemic. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 119, Article 103440. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvb.2020.103440

Rettie, H., & Daniels, J. (2021). Coping and tolerance of uncertainty: Predictors and mediators of mental health during the COVID-19 pandemic. American Psychologist, 76(3), 427–437. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000710

Rodriguez, L. M., Neighbors, C., Rinker, D. V., & Tackett, J. L. (2015). Motivational profiles of gambling behavior: Self-determination theory, gambling motives, and gambling behavior. Journal of Gambling Studies, 31(4), 1597–1615. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-014-9497-7

Rohsenow, D. J., Monti, P. M., Martin, R. A., Colby, S. M., Myers, M. G., Gulliver, S. B., Brown, R. A., Mueller, T. I., Gordon, A., & Abrams, D. B. (2004). Motivational enhancement and coping skills training for cocaine abusers: Effects on substance use outcomes. Addiction, 99(7), 862–874. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2004.00743.x

Rozgonjuk, D., Pontes, H. M., Schivinski, B., & Montag, C. (2022). Disordered gaming, loneliness, and family harmony in gamers before and during the COVID-19 pandemic. Addictive Behaviors Reports, 15, Article 100426. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.abrep.2022.100426

Sallie, S. N., Ritou, V. J. E., Bowden-Jones, H., & Voon, V. (2021). Assessing online gaming and pornography consumption patterns during COVID-19 isolation using an online survey: Highlighting distinct avenues of problematic internet behavior. Addictive Behaviors, 123, Article 107044. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2021.107044

Sanders, J. L., Williams, R. J., & Damgaard, M. (2017). Video game play and internet gaming disorder among Canadian adults: A national survey. Canadian Journal of Addiction, 8(2), 6–12. https://doi.org/10.1097/CXA.0000000000000006

Sartorius, N., Ustün, T. B., Lecrubier, Y., & Wittchen, H. U. (1996). Depression comorbid with anxiety: Results from the WHO study on psychological disorders in primary health care. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 168(30), 38–43. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8864147

Shah, S. M. A., Mohammad, D., Qureshi, M. F. H., Abbas, M. Z., & Aleem, S. (2021). Prevalence, psychological responses and associated correlates of depression, anxiety and stress in a global population, during the coronavirus disease (COVID-19) pandemic. Community Mental Health Journal, 57(1), 101–110. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10597-020-00728-y

Sobell, L. C., & Sobell, M. B. (1992). Timeline follow-back. In R. Z. Litten & J. P. Allen (Eds.), Measuring alcohol consumption (pp. 41–72). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-0357-5_3

Spitzer, R. L., Kroenke, K., Williams, J. B. W., & Löwe, B. (2006). A brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder: The GAD-7. Archives of Internal Medicine, 166(10), 1092–1097. https://doi.org/10.1001/archinte.166.10.1092

Stevens, M. W., Dorstyn, D., Delfabbro, P. H., & King, D. L. (2021). Global prevalence of gaming disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 55(6), 553–568. https://doi.org/10.1177/0004867420962851

Teng, Z., Pontes, H. M., Nie, Q., Griffiths, M. D., & Guo, C. (2021). Depression and anxiety symptoms associated with internet gaming disorder before and during the COVID-19 pandemic: A longitudinal study. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 10(1), 169–180. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.2021.00016

Terlecki, M., Brown, J., Harner-Steciw, L., Irvin-Hannum, J., Marchetto-Ryan, N., Ruhl, L., & Wiggins, J. (2011). Sex differences and similarities in video game experience, preferences, and self-efficacy: Implications for the gaming industry. Current Psychology, 30(1), 22–33. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-010-9095-5

The Canadian Press. (2020). A provincial guide to what’s being done to fight COVID-19. OHS. https://www.ohscanada.com/provincial-guide-whats-done-fight-covid-19/

Vadlin, S., Åslund, C., Hellström, C., & Nilsson, K. W. (2016). Associations between problematic gaming and psychiatric symptoms among adolescents in two samples. Addictive Behaviors, 61, 8–15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2016.05.001

van der Velden, P. G., Contino, C., Das, M., van Loon, P., & Bosmans, M. W. G. (2020). Anxiety and depression symptoms, and lack of emotional support among the general population before and during the COVID-19 pandemic. A prospective national study on prevalence and risk factors. Journal of Affective Disorders, 277, 540–548. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2020.08.026

Viana, R. B., & de Lira, C. A. B. (2020). Exergames as coping strategies for anxiety disorders during the COVID-19 quarantine period. Games for Health Journal, 9(3), 147–149. https://doi.org/10.1089/g4h.2020.0060

Wang, C., Pan, R., Wan, X., Tan, Y., Xu, L., Ho, C. S., & Ho, R. C. (2020). Immediate psychological responses and associated factors during the initial stage of the 2019 coronavirus disease (COVID-19) epidemic among the general population in China. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(5), Article 1729. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17051729

Wang, C.-Y., Wu, Y.-C., Su, C.-H., Lin, P.-C., Ko, C.-H., & Yen, J.-Y. (2017). Association between internet gaming disorder and generalized anxiety disorder. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 6(4), 564–571. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.6.2017.088

Wardell, J. D., Kempe, T., Rapinda, K. K., Single, A., Bilevicius, E., Frohlich, J. R., Hendershot, C. S., & Keough, M. T. (2020). Drinking to cope during COVID-19 pandemic: The role of external and internal factors in coping motive pathways to alcohol use, solitary drinking, and alcohol problems. Alcohol: Clinical and Experimental Research, 44(10), 2073–2083. https://doi.org/10.1111/acer.14425

Wartberg, L., Kriston, L., Kramer, M., Schwedler, A., Lincoln, T. M., & Kammerl, R. (2017). Internet gaming disorder in early adolescence: Associations with parental and adolescent mental health. Europian Psychiatry, 43, 14–18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2016.12.013

Weinstein, N., Przybylski, A. K., & Murayama, K. (2017). A prospective study of the motivational and health dynamics of internet gaming disorder. PeerJ, 5, Article e3838. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.3838

Weinstock, J., Whelan, J. P., & Meyers, A. W. (2004). Behavioral assessment of gambling: An application of the timeline followback method. Psychological Assessment, 16(1), 72–80. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.16.1.72

Williams, N. (2014). The GAD-7 questionnaire. Occupational Medicine, 64(3), 224–224. https://doi.org/10.1093/occmed/kqt161

Wisener, M., & Khoury, B. (2021). Specific emotion-regulation processes explain the relationship between mindfulness and self-compassion with coping-motivated alcohol and marijuana use. Addictive Behaviors, 112, Article 106590. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2020.106590

World Health Organization (2019). International statistical classification of diseases and related health problems (11th ed.). https://icd.who.int/

Xu, S., Park, M., Kang, U. G., Choi, J.-S., & Koo, J. W. (2021). Problematic use of alcohol and online gaming as coping strategies during the COVID-19 pandemic: A mini review. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 12, Article 930. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.685964

Additional information

Authors' Contribution

Rebecca Lewinson: writing—original draft, writing—review & editing, formal analysis. Jeffrey D. Wardell: conceptualization, methodology, writing—review & editing. Naama Kronstein: data curation, project administration, investigation, writing—review & editing. Karli K. Rapinda: data curation, project administration, investigation, writing—review & editing. Tyler Kempe: data curation, project administration, investigation. Joel Katz: supervision, writing—review & editing. Hyoun S. Kim: methodology, writing—review & editing. Matthew T. Keough: supervision, writing—review & editing, formal analysis, conceptualization, methodology.

Editorial Record

First submission received:
September 10, 2022

Revisions received:
March 6, 2023
April 21, 2023

Accepted for publication:
May 4, 2023

Editor in charge:
Maèva Flayelle

Full text

Introduction

Background

The Coronavirus disease of 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic brought with it public health efforts to reduce its spread, including mandated lockdowns and physical distancing, resulting in fewer opportunities for social interaction and for effective coping (Restubog et al., 2020; Rettie & Daniels, 2021). Preliminary data from early stages of the pandemic demonstrated that people from many countries experienced higher levels of distress, anxiety, and depression than they had prior to the pandemic (Alyami et al., 2021; Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2020; Fountoulakis et al., 2021; Hyland et al., 2020; Shah et al., 2021; C. Wang et al., 2020). Given that the COVID-19 pandemic hindered social interactions for the greater part of two years, individuals may have sought solitary coping methods to deal with their isolation and negative emotions. Studies have demonstrated that the ongoing disruptions caused by COVID-19 include reduced social connectedness, along with disruptions to psychological and physical well-being (Krause et al., 2022; McMahon et al., 2022). Along with the chronicity of these disruptions, new stressors have emerged relating to these disruptions, including the repeated re-opening and closures of businesses and recreational activities, chronic work and family stress, and the emergence of new variants (Calvano et al., 2022; Jain & Jolly, 2021; Mohanty et al., 2022). A study conducted by Gillen et al. (2022) examined cross-sectional data of UK participants at three time points throughout the pandemic (May–July 2020; November 2020–January 2021; May–July 2021). The researchers found that overall well-being decreased between the first and second time points. Moreover, the researchers also found that negative coping strategies such as substance use, and behavioural disengagement increased between the first and last time points. These results suggest that individuals may be adopting riskier coping techniques to manage these disruptions and stressors.

Video games are one such coping mechanism that individuals may have turned to during the COVID-19 pandemic. Prior to COVID-19, over 23 million Canadians (61% of Canadians) identified themselves as “gamers” (Entertainment Software Association of Canada, 2018). Though this number did not change at the beginning of the pandemic (Entertainment Software Association of Canada, 2020), 58% of adult gamers and 80% of teen gamers have endorsed gaming more regularly during COVID-19 (Entertainment Software Association of Canada, 2020). A few studies have shown an increase in gaming since the start of the COVID-19 pandemic. In America, Verizon reported a 75% increase in online gaming activity that corresponds with stay-at-home directives, whereas Italy saw a 70% increase in internet traffic related to Fortnite, a popular online video game (King et al., 2020). India also saw a 30% increase in online mobile gaming and a 35% increase in multi-player games (Amin et al., 2022). Live stream gaming such as YouTube Gaming and Twitch have also reported a 10% increase in audiences (López-Cabarcos et al., 2020), indicating that people had turned not only to gaming during the pandemic, but also to other gaming-related activities. While gaming is not an inherently problem activity, approximately 3.2% individuals who game go on to develop Gaming Disorder (Petry & O’Brien, 2013; Przybylski et al., 2017; Sanders et al., 2017; Stevens et al., 2021). People with pre-existing vulnerabilities, like mood and anxiety disorders, are particularly susceptible to developing Gaming Disorder (Fazeli et al., 2020; King et al., 2019; Viana & de Lira, 2020).

Gaming Disorder is a formalized diagnosis found in the International Classification of Diseases, 11th Revision (ICD-11; World Health Organization, 2019) that is characterized by impaired control over gaming habits, as well as an escalation of gaming despite related problems (Jo et al., 2019). In contrast, “Internet Gaming Disorder” (IGD) has been included in section III of the Diagnostic and Statistical manual—Fifth edition (DSM-5; APA, 2013) as a disorder for future consideration by the American Psychiatric Association. For the purposes of this paper, the term “Gaming Disorder” will be used to encompass the list of symptoms described in the DSM-5. Although its name suggests that it encompasses only internet gaming, IGD encapsulates both internet- and non-internet-based gaming behaviours (APA, 2013; Kuss et al., 2017). Finally, excessive gaming is a term used to conceptualize addictive behaviour related to gaming, without a formal diagnosis being attached to the definition (Sanders et al., 2017).

Gaming Disorder has been associated with a number of other mental health concerns including anxiety
(C.-Y. Wang et al., 2017), depression (Liu et al., 2018), and substance use (Na et al., 2017). This is particularly important to consider, given that anxiety and depression have been linked to increases in problematic gaming behaviour (Männikkö et al., 2020). During the COVID-19 pandemic, the proportion of Canadians who self-reported high or extremely high anxiety quadrupled (from 5% to 20%), and rates of high depression symptoms more than doubled (from 4% to 10%; Dozois, 2021). Several studies from outside North America have demonstrated that COVID-19-related increases in mood and anxiety symptoms were positively associated with problematic gaming (Fazeli et al., 2020; Teng et al., 2021). A longitudinal study conducted by Teng et al. (2021) collected self-reported survey data from 1,778 children and adolescents from Southwest China at two time points; one prior to the COVID-19 pandemic (October–November 2019), and one during the COVID-19 pandemic (April–May 2020). Participants reported more time spent video gaming, as well as increases in anxiety and depression from time one to time two. Moreover, cross-lagged modeling showed that pre-pandemic anxiety and depressive symptoms predicted both Internet Gaming Disorder and time spent gaming during the pandemic. The authors also found that there was an uptick in the incidence of Gaming Disorder post-pandemic. Fazeli et al. (2020) examined the association of the pandemic on adolescents’ mental health and gaming behaviours in Iran using a cross-sectional study. In this study, 1,512 Iranian adolescent participants were asked to complete self-report questionnaires between May–August 2020 surrounding symptoms of IGD, depression, anxiety, and stress. The results showed strong positive associations between depression, anxiety, stress, and gaming-related problems.

Motivational theory has been a long-standing framework for understanding addictive behaviours; namely, that motivational types have been studied extensively in relation to alcohol (Cooper et al., 1995; Cox & Klinger, 1988; Miller et al., 2000) and other substance use (Barnett et al., 2012; Cooper et al., 2016; Rohsenow et al., 2004), but also in relation to other addictive behaviours such as gambling (Chantal & Vallerand, 1996; Rodriguez et al., 2015) or gaming (Przybylski & Weinstein, 2019; Weinstein et al., 2017). Similarly, according to motivational models of addiction (Cooper, 1994; Myrseth et al., 2017), people who are emotionally vulnerable are at risk for excessive gaming and related harms (Balhara et al., 2018). These people may have been the ones who increased their gaming in efforts to cope with the pandemic situation, resulting in greater gaming-related problems. While some literature links anxiety and depressive symptoms to the onset of Gaming Disorder during the pandemic (Teng et al., 2021), longer-term longutudinal studies have not been conducted to examine these associations and the underlying mechanisms in the general population of North Americans during the COVID-19 situation.

Gaming motives are proximal cognitive factors that are believed to mediate the effects of individual differences (e.g., anxiety and depression) on excessive gaming and related harms (Király et al., 2015; Marino et al., 2020). Myrseth et al. (2017) identified four key motives for gaming, namely: enhancement motives (internal, positive reinforcement; gaming for the pleasurable experience of gaming itself), coping motives (internal, negative reinforcement; reduction of negative emotions), social motives (external, positive reinforcement motives; increasing social interaction), and self-gratification motives (gaming to satisfy one’s own personal desires). Coping motives seem to be the most central to problematic gaming, particularly among those with emotional vulnerabilities (Myrseth et al., 2017). While enhancement motivations tend to predict greater engagement in gaming activities, coping motivations have been found to predict increased risk for gaming harms. Furthermore, coping motives independently predict a loss of control of gaming behaviours as well as the development of gaming problems (Myrseth et al., 2017).

A more recent systematic review also found that coping motives, along with avoidance motives, not only predict the development of Internet Gaming Disorder, but that these motives also mediate the role between psychological factors (e.g., anxiety, loneliness, or self-esteem) and problematic gaming behaviours (Melodia et al., 2020). A further study conducted by Caro and Popovac (2021) found that individuals who turned to gaming during difficult life experiences also experienced more maladaptive emotional regulation along with lower coping self-efficacy, with coping self-efficacy being a significant predictor of gaming during difficult life circumstances.

These data suggest that coping motives may well be the most salient cognitive mechanism explaining the effects of depression and anxiety on pandemic-related increases in gaming and related harms among emotionally vulnerable individuals. To our knowledge, no previous studies have explored how coping motives for gaming are linked to excessive gaming during the COVID-19 pandemic among emotionally vulnerable people using a longitudinal design.

The Present Study

This three-time point design was used with Canadian participants completing survey measures via Prolific approximately three months apart beginning after the COVID-19 emergency was declared (on March 17, 2020). Our primary goal was to understand how emotional vulnerability early in the pandemic was prospectively related to coping-motivated gaming and related problems six months into the COVID-19 pandemic. Currently, very little is known about how gaming habits and related problems have changed over the course of the pandemic, and our goal is to fill this critical gap in the literature. Several studies have examined cross-sectional or short-term associations between emotional vulnerability (i.e., depression and anxiety) and problematic gaming during the COVID-19 pandemic (Fazeli et al., 2020; Teng et al., 2021); however, our study is novel in several ways. It is the only study to examine temporal pathways from emotional vulnerability to gaming during the first six months of the COVID-19 pandemic using a multi-wave longitudinal design, giving insight to how these pathways or variables might change over time. Second, previous studies have not focused on coping motives as a primary mediator of emotional vulnerability-gaming associations during the pandemic. Finally, the present study applied a path model to both sexes (male vs. female) and in larger or smaller provinces (e.g., Ontario vs. all other provinces) using invariance testing.

For the current study, we hypothesized that higher levels of emotional vulnerability (i.e., anxiety and depression) in the early stages of the pandemic (Time 1) would prospectively predict greater time spent gaming and related problems at six-months into the pandemic (Time 3). We also hypothesized that increasing coping motives for gaming (Time 2) would explain these effects. Invariance tests were also conducted for location, particularly Ontario versus all other provinces, and for sex (male versus female). A majority of our sample originated from Ontario, and given the regional differences in lockdown procedures across Canada (Plett et al., 2022) including changes in economic activity (McCormack, 2021), freedom of movement, masking and social distancing regulations, and capacity limitations (Cameron-Blake et al., 2021). Previous studies have found sex differences in the way and amount that men and women game, their preferences for gaming, as well as their confidence in their gaming ability (Lange & Schwab, 2018; Lucas & Sherry, 2004; Terlecki et al., 2011). Furthermore, Gaming Disorders are more prevalent in males (Marraudino et al., 2022). It is therefore important to ensure that the hypothesized pathways do not differ across sex or location so that we are able to ensure that the interpretations of our results are consistent across our sample. We did not have a suitable sample size of participants who identified as non-binary, and as such, these participants were excluded from our analysis.

Methods     

Participants and Procedure

The data for this research was derived from a larger longitudinal study on addictive behaviours during the COVID-19 pandemic (Baptist Mohseni et al., 2022; Wardell et al., 2020). This research was reviewed and approved by the York University Research Ethics Board (Human Participants Review Committee certificate #e2020-118). Participants were recruited through Prolific, a web-based survey platform purposefully designed for the scientific community to conduct research (Palan & Schitter, 2018). Participants were Canadian adult gamers who reported gaming within the past three months prior to the time of baseline data collection. It should be noted that all participants who endorsed gaming at baseline were included in the analysis, even if they reported no gaming at the follow up surveys. Participants also needed to have a history of high-quality responses on the Prolific platform. These participants were taken from a larger sample of participants who also self-identified as regular drinkers of alcoholic beverages. The sample consisted of 332 self-reported gamers (Mage= 33.79, SDage=8.92, 60.7% male, 39.2% female), with most of the participants (64.5%) identifying as being White. A majority of these participants (86.2%) reported being employed prior to the COVID-19 pandemic and reported stable or increased income (56.3%) since the start of the pandemic. Approximately 91% of our sample self-identified as being at high-risk for COVID-19, and spent less than one hour (80.4%) per day watching COVID-19 related news. Most of our participants did not live alone (66.6%) and had children (72.9%). Four attention check items (Prolific, 2018) were also included to ensure that the participants were responding conscientiously. Participants were excluded from the analysis if they failed two or more attention check items. Two participants were excluded from the analysis as a result of meeting these criteria. Data collection took place as follows: end of April/beginning of May 2020 (Time 1), July 2020 (Time 2), and October 2020 (Time 3). Participants answered the questionnaires based on the 30-days prior to each survey. The baseline data collection took place 1–2 months after a state of emergency was declared across Canada (occurring between March 12 and March 22, 2020, depending on location; The Canadian Press, 2020). Participants were compensated $13 CAD at each time point.

Measures

Table 1 outlines the timeline of the study as well as which measures were used at each time point.

General Anxiety Disorder-7 (GAD-7)

The GAD-7 is a 7-item brief self-report measure of anxiety symptoms. It measures anxiety symptoms over the past two weeks (e.g., Over the last two weeks how often have you been bothered by…feeling nervous, anxious, or on edge). It has been validated in both clinical and non-clinical populations (Löwe et al., 2008; Spitzer et al., 2006; Williams, 2014). Participants respond to items on a 0 (not at all) to 3 (nearly every day) point Likert Scale. Higher scores are associated with a higher severity of symptoms of GAD. A score of 0–4 is considered “minimal anxiety”, 5–9 is considered “mild anxiety”, 10–14 is considered “moderate anxiety”, and a score of 15–21 is considered “severe anxiety”. The GAD-7 has been shown to have good criterion and construct validity, along with excellent internal consistency (α = .92; Spitzer et al., 2006). The internal consistency of the GAD-7 for the present study was α = .91 at Time 1.

 Table 1. Synopsis of Measures Used at Time 1, Time 2, and Time 3.

 

Time 1

(May 2020)

Time 2

(July 2020)

Time 3

(October 2020)

Measures used

GAD-7

PHQ-9

EGMQ

TLFB

IGDS-SF9

GAD-7

PHQ-9

EMGQ

TLFB

GAD-7

PHQ-9

EMGQ

TLFB

Note. GAD-7: Generalized Anxiety Disorder-7; PHQ-9: Patient Health Questionnaire; EGMQ: Electronic Gaming Motives Questionnaire; TLFB: Timeline Follow-back; IGDS-SF9: Internet Gaming Disorder Scale, Short Form.

Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-9)

The PHQ-9 is a 9-item brief self-report measure of depressive symptoms occurring over the past two weeks (e.g., Over the last 2 weeks, how often have you been bothered by…feeling tired or having little energy). It has been validated in both clinical and non-clinical populations (Doi et al., 2018). It utilizes the nine DSM-5 criteria for depression, with each item being rated on a 0 (not at all) to 3 (nearly every day) point Likert scale (Kroenke & Spitzer, 2002; Kroenke et al., 2001). Scores ranging between 1–4 are considered “minimal depression, while scores between 5–9 are reflective of “mild depression”, 10–14 are considered “moderate depression”, 15–19 are considered “moderately severe depression”, and scores between 20–27 are considered to be “severe depression”. The PHQ-9 has been shown to have good construct and criterion validity along with excellent internal consistency (α = .89; Kroenke et al., 2001). The internal consistency of the PHQ-9 for the present study was α = .87 at Time 1.

Electronic Gaming Motives Questionnaire (EGMQ)

The EGMQ is a self-report measure of video gaming motives (Myrseth et al., 2017). It consists of 14 items measuring four motives: enhancement (e.g., because you like the feeling), coping (e.g., to forget your worries), social motives (e.g., because it makes a social gathering more enjoyable), and self-gratification motives (e.g., as a way to celebrate). Participants respond to the items on a scale ranging from 1 (almost never/never) to 4 (almost always) (Myrseth et al., 2017). The EGMQ has been shown to have good criterion validity, and good internal consistency (α = .67 to .84; Myrseth et al., 2017). The internal consistency in our sample for baseline coping motives was α = .80 at baseline and α = .81 at Time 2. The internal consistency in our sample for baseline enhancement motives was α = .77 at baseline and α = .80 at Time 2.

Timeline Follow-Back (TLFB)

Though the TLFB method was initially created to assess for alcohol use (Sobell & Sobell, 1992), it has also been successfully used to measure gambling-related behaviours (Weinstock et al., 2004), substance use (Agrawal et al., 2008), and video gaming (Peter et al., 2020) in previous studies. In the current study, participants completed an online, self-report version of the TLFB. Participants used the TLFB method to measure the time spent gaming (in hours) at the three time points of data collection. At each time point, the time spent gaming is based on the week prior to completing the survey (e.g., To help us evaluate your game use, we need to get an idea of your game use was like in an average week in the past month (30 days). To do this, we would like you to fill out the calendar below). Participants were asked to fill in the number of hours that they typically spent gaming on each day of the week over the past month. The number of hours spent gaming per day were then summed across the days.

Internet Gaming Disorder Scale—Short Form (IGDS-SF9)

The IGDS-SF9 scale is a 9-item self-report measure that was adapted using the nine core DSM-5 criteria that defines Internet Gaming Disorder (APA, 2013). Items are rated on a 1 (never) to 5 (very often) Likert-type scale (e.g., Do you feel more irritability, anxiety, or even sadness when you try to either reduce or stop your gaming activity?), with higher scores being indicative of higher levels of Internet Gaming Disorder (Lemmens et al., 2015). It has been shown to have good internal consistency (α = .83), along with good criterion and concurrent validity (Lemmens et al., 2015; Pontes & Griffiths, 2015). The internal consistency of the IDGS-SF9 in our sample ranged from α = .90 and α = .91.

Data Analysis Overview

We conducted preliminary analyses prior to hypothesis testing with path modelling. This involved data screening (i.e., winsorizing extreme values to + 3.29 standard deviations from the mean and verifying multiple regression assumptions) and conducting a missing data analysis. For the missing data, we used a series of t-tests to examine potential baseline differences between participants with complete data versus those with incomplete data (Enders, 2010). This was done to determine the nature of data loss. Next, we used Structural Equation Modeling (SEM) in MPlus v7.4 to test the hypothesized model (Muthén & Muthén, 2017).

In the main SEM analysis, we modelled emotional vulnerability as a latent predictor, with anxiety and depression as indicator variables. It has been previously demonstrated that excessive gaming is positively associated with symptoms of depression and anxiety (Ballabio et al., 2017; Fazeli et al., 2020; Kim et al., 2016; Teng et al., 2021). Depression and Anxiety have been shown in several studies to be highly comorbid with one another (Gorman, 1996; Hirschfeld, 2001; Pollack, 2005; Sartorius et al., 1996). Given this high comorbidity, the current literature has suggested that anxiety and depression should be conceptualized as one transdiagnostic factor rather than separate entities (Brown & Barlow, 2009; Craske, 2012). Given this association, we captured this underlying dimension of depression and anxiety together by creating a latent predictor, emotional vulnerability, using scores from the Generalized Anxiety Disorder Scale (GAD-7), and the Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-9) to calculate the shared variance between the two. Emotional vulnerability at Time 1 was specified as the main predictor; coping and enhancement motives at Time 2 (controlling for Time 1) were entered as correlated mediators; and time spent gaming and related problems at Time 3 (controlling for Time 1) were specified as correlated outcomes. We opted to include only internal motives for gaming in the main SEM analysis based on the literature showing that these motives are generally associated with greater gaming-related harms relative to external motives (Ballabio et al., 2017; Hellström et al., 2015; Kuss et al., 2012; Myrseth et al., 2017). Including enhancement motives in the model also allowed us to demonstrate the specificity of coping motives as a hypothesized mediator. Overall, our specified longitudinal SEM analysis allowed us to establish complete temporal precedence among predictors, mediators, and outcomes, which strengthened our interpretation of mediational effects.

Fit of the hypothesized model was evaluated using several indices. Fit was considered excellent if the following guidelines were met: a χ2/df ratio < 3.0 (Kline, 2010); a root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) <.06; a comparative fit index (CFI) >.95; and a standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) <.08 (Hu & Bentler, 1999) In addition, the precision and reliability of direct and indirect paths were evaluated using a bootstrapped bias-corrected 95% Confidence Interval (CI) approach (Fritz & MacKinnon, 2007). If the 95% CI for a given direct or indirect path coefficient does not include zero, the effect is considered to be supported (Fritz & Mackinnon, 2007; Hu & Bentler, 1999; Kline, 2013). Full information maximum likelihood (FIML) was used to handle missing data and Maximum Likelihood Ratio (MLR) was used to obtain fit indices, given the non-normality of the gaming outcomes (Muthén & Muthén, 2012). Following the main SEM, the invariance of our model was tested across biological sex (i.e., men vs. women) and Canadian province (Ontario vs. other provinces). Approximately 51% of our sample originated from Ontario, which was a province that had quick and widespread lockdowns, along with other provincial guidelines. In addition, Ontario is the largest province in terms of population size and density. As such, we wanted to ensure that the model was similar in Ontario versus the rest of Canada.

In order to proceed with the path invariance testing, the proposed model was first tested in each sex or provincial group individually to ensure good fit. A configural model was then tested, one that allows the paths to vary freely between groups. Assuming that good model fit was established in these first two steps, a path invariance model was estimated that constrains the direct effects to be equal effects across sex groups or provincial groups, accordingly. If there were no significant differences in model fit between the configural and invariant models, then it was inferred that the overall model applies equally across sex (i.e., to both males and females) or provincial (i.e., to both Ontario and other provinces) groups. Differences in fit between path invariant and configural models were evaluated using the Δχ2 test and the change in CFI value. A significant difference between models is supported if the p-value for the Δχ2 test is below .05 and/or the ΔCFI is ≥ .01 (Cheung & Rensvold, 2002).

Results

Descriptive Statistics

Table 2 shows the descriptive data for the observed variables. The mean score on the IGDS-SF9 suggested that the Gaming Disorder symptoms in our sample were well below the clinical cut-off of 36 (Pontes & Griffiths, 2015) that would be indicative of disordered gaming behaviours. Our participants spent on average approximately 20 hours per week (SD = 16.08) gaming at the beginning of the study (~2.86 hours per day), and approximately 13 hours per week (SD = 14.55) gaming at Time 3 (~1.86 hours per day). The scores for both the PHQ-9 and the GAD-7 were indicative of endorsement of mild symptoms of depression and anxiety, respectively. Despite the mean being low in this sample, we did observe a wide range of gaming activity, and symptoms of anxiety and depression. As such, this gave us variability in order to allow us to analyze our SEM model.

Table 2. Descriptive Statistics for the Observed Variables.

Variable

Mean

Median

SD

Range

Gaming Problems (T1)

15.13

13

6.38

9–45

Gaming Problems (T3)

14.07

12

6.00

9–37

Time Spent Gaming (T1)

19.95

17

16.08

0–164

Time Spent Gaming (T3)

13.37

9

14.55

0–111

PHQ-9 (T1)

7.81

7

5.32

0–27

GAD-7 (T1)

5.96

6

4.67

0–21

Coping Motives (T1)

9.50

9

2.85

4–16

Coping Motives (T2)

8.90

8

2.94

4–16

Enhancement Motives (T1)

8.86

9

2.24

3–12

Enhancement Motives (T2)

8.69

9

2.37

3–12

Of the original sample, a total of n = 264 completed the second time point, and n = 228 completed the third time point. T-tests examining baseline differences between completers (coded as 1; n = 228) and non-completers (coded as 0; n = 104) did not reveal significant differences on any measures included in the main SEM model: PHQ-9, t(330) = −0.13, p = .90, GAD-7, t(330) = −1.07, = .29, time spent gaming, t(330) = 0.82, p = .42, gaming problems,
t(330) = 0.31, = .75, coping motives, t(330) = −1.62, p = .11, or enhancement motives, t(330) = −.91, p = .36.

Mediation Model

Model Results

The fit of our original hypothesized model was excellent: χ2= 36.65, df = 21, p = .018, χ2/df = 1.74; CFI = .982, RMSEA = .047, 95% CI [.019, .072], SRMR = .036. The coefficients and 95% CIs for the direct paths are shown in Figure 1. As hypothesized, high levels of emotional vulnerability at baseline predicted elevated coping motives at Time 2, controlling for coping motives at Time 1. The effect of emotional vulnerability on enhancement at Time 2, controlling for enhancement motives at Time 1, was not statistically significant. Coping motives at Time 2 were significant positive predictors of both gaming problems and time spent gaming at Time 3, controlling for baseline gaming behaviours. Enhancement motives at Time 2 were not a significant predictor of gaming problems or time spent gaming at Time 3, after controlling for baseline gaming behaviours. Next, indirect effects were inspected. As hypothesized, there were supported mediational effects from emotional vulnerability (Time 1), via coping motives (Time 2), to both gaming problems β = .018, 95% CI [0.001, 0.055], and time spent gaming β = .016 95% CI [.001, .047] at Time 3. No mediational effects were observed from emotional vulnerability to gaming behaviours via enhancement motives, suggesting specificity through coping motivations. Overall, our findings show that elevated depression and anxiety early in the pandemic relate to future coping-motivated gaming and associated harms.

Next, the invariance of the proposed model was tested across sex (male vs. female), as well as location (Ontario vs. other provinces). The fit and model values for these invariance tests can be found in Table 3. The model fit the data well for males and for females. As seen in Table 3, there was no significant difference between the configural and path invariant models, suggesting that the effects in our path model did not differ between the sex groups. Similar results were obtained for invariance testing across provinces (see Table 3), suggesting that the hypothesized model was applicable to individuals living across Canada.

Table 3. Invariance Testing by Sex and by Province: Model Fit Information.

Model Type

χ2

df

p value

CFI

RMSEA

SRMR

χ2 difference

df

p value

CFI difference

Sex

Overall

36.65

21

.018

.982

.047

.036

 

 

 

 

Male

16.50

21

.741

1.000

.000

.026

 

 

 

 

Female

37.51

21

.014

.951

.060

.054

 

 

 

 

Configural

54.02

44

.143

.988

.037

.040

 

 

 

 

Path Invariance

66.27

50

.061

.981

.044

.048

12.25

6

.056

.007

Province

Overall

36.65

21

.018

.982

.047

.036

 

 

 

 

Ontario

24.24

21

.281

.993

.030

.044

 

 

 

 

Rest of Canada

31.41

21

.067

.974

.055

.049

 

 

 

 

Configural

61.61

44

.041

.980

.049

.045

 

 

 

 

Path Invariance

66.79

50

.056

.981

.045

.048

5.18

6

.521

.001

Note. Model cut-offs are as follows: Chi-square (p >.05), CFI ≥.95, RMSEA ≤.06, SRMR ≤ .08, chi-squared change (p < .05) and CFI difference < .01.

Discussion

This study is the first to longitudinally examine the motivational mechanisms underlying the relationship between emotional vulnerability and gaming-related problems during the COVID-19 pandemic. We found that participants who had higher levels of emotional vulnerability tended to use gaming as a form of coping with that emotional distress.

Moreover, our research demonstrated that those with greater coping motives for gaming at three months into the pandemic also endorsed more gaming problems at six-months into the pandemic along with more time spent gaming. This suggests that during the COVID-19 pandemic, individuals may have been turning to gaming in an attempt to cope with their emotional vulnerability or their newfound social isolation. Furthermore, this suggests that some individuals were not gaming for the simple pleasure of gaming in and of itself and may have been attempting to use gaming as a method of reducing their emotional distress. This model seemed to apply to both males and females, and those across Canada, as evidenced by the invariance testing that was conducted.

These findings are consistent with previous research, showing that those who report higher levels of stress and anxiety also tend to demonstrate an increase in passive coping styles such as escapism (Fu et al., 2020) and that coping motives are strongly associated with a heightened risk of problematic addictive behaviours (Moitra et al., 2015; Wisener & Khoury, 2021). These findings are particularly important when considering the context of the COVID-19 pandemic. There are established links between emotional vulnerability and problematic gaming (Balhara et al., 2018); however, the motivational mechanisms of this association, particularly during the pandemic, have not been described prior to this study (Fazeli et al., 2020; Teng et al., 2021). Research has shown that those who are emotionally vulnerable were disproportionately affected by the pandemic (Panteli et al., 2022; van der Velden et al., 2020), and these individuals may have used gaming excessively as a way to distract themselves from those negative emotions (King et al., 2020; Teng et al., 2021). Although many individuals use gaming as a positive coping method to mitigate their stress, anxiety, depression, and loneliness (Barr & Copeland-Stewart, 2022; Pallavicini et al., 2022), for individuals who were at-risk for addictive behaviour (e.g., male youths or those with maladaptive or avoidant coping styles), excessive gaming has been found to have long-term increases in symptoms of anxiety, depression, and overall stress, but short-term relaxing effects to the player (Pallavicini et al., 2022).

 

 Figure 1. Structural Equation Model of Emotional Vulnerability to Gaming Behaviours Through Coping and Enhancement Gaming Motivations.

 

Note. Grey arrows represent non-significant pathways, while black arrows denote significant pathways. Standardized estimates are shown with their corresponding 95% confidence intervals. The covariances among all T1 variables were included in the model but omitted from the figure.

 

This relationship may be further explained by the self-medication hypothesis, which states that individuals who are more emotionally vulnerable and are at risk for gaming excessively do so to cope with their emotional distress (Balhara et al., 2018; González-Bueso et al., 2018; King & Delfabbro, 2016; Liu et al., 2018; Vadlin et al., 2016; Wartberg et al., 2017). Within the context of the COVID-19 pandemic, studies have found that those who experience heightened stress, depression, or anxiety due to social isolation or loneliness, also tend to experience excessive gaming behaviours (Rozgonjuk et al., 2022; Sallie et al., 2021; Xu et al., 2021). Furthermore, previous research has also found higher emotional distress among those who game excessively during the COVID-19 pandemic (Giardina et al., 2021). Therefore, it may be that these individuals are gaming as a means to cope with their emotional distress.      

The results of this study may help to determine how gaming is being used during the pandemic as a coping method and may subsequently inform interventions relating to problematic gaming or pandemic-related coping methods. Given that coping motives seem to be associated with increased problematic gaming, it may be beneficial to incorporate coping motives for gaming and emotional vulnerability into interventions designed to curb the incidence or severity of Gaming Disorder, particularly within the context of the COVID-19 pandemic. In particular, it may be beneficial for interventions to focus on establishing or strengthening coping mechanisms or reducing pre-existing emotional vulnerability. This may be an area for future research.

This study had some limitations. Firstly, the participants in this study were Canadian; given that the data came only from one country, the results may not necessarily be generalizable to other countries. Secondly, given the convenience recruitment of the participants through Prolific, an online survey platform, and that we do not have comprehensive pre-pandemic information on the participants’ gaming behaviours, we are unable to speak to changes in gaming from pre- to post-pandemic, and can only interpret the findings of changes during the pandemic. Furthermore, participants all were self-reported regular drinkers of alcoholic beverages. It may be beneficial to repeat this study with a participant pool from the general population that includes people who do not drink as well, to increase generalizability of our findings. The socioeconomic status of our sample is also relatively high, and most of our participants did not live alone which may limit the generalizability of results to more diverse socioeconomic populations or those with higher levels of isolation. In addition, our sample was largely subclinical in their gaming behaviours and emotional vulnerability. As such, while our results are consistent with theories of gaming behaviour, this study should be replicated and extended to populations with more severe emotional vulnerability and gaming problems. Future studies should aim to replicate this study using larger and more diverse samples and may wish to consider COVID-specific contextual factors such as incidence rate in the participants’ location and adherence to governmental guidelines regarding physical distancing. Furthermore, future studies should aim to consider using measures that have been validated for COVID-19-specific concerns and behaviours.

Conclusion

These findings aid in our understanding of gaming behaviours and gaming motivations during the COVID-19 pandemic. This longitudinal study provides information not only to how individuals were coping with the extreme changes that have occurred due to the pandemic and governmental guidelines, but also how emotional vulnerability may have influenced their gaming behaviours. Given that since the time of data collection there have been significant changes- most notably, the introduction of COVID-19 vaccines- it will be important to determine how these coping motivations and gaming behaviours are altered as the pandemic continued. It has been suggested that the impacts of the pandemic on addictive behaviours will be long-term given the increased psychological distress that accompanies the pandemic (Rehm et al., 2020), and it will be important to investigate these long-term impacts.

Conflict of Interest

The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.

 

Metrics

0

Crossref logo

0

0

web of science logo


1125

Views

434

PDF views