Social identification and collective action participation in the internet age: A meta-analysis

Vol.15,No.4(2021)

Abstract

Since the digitally-mediated large-scale protests took place all over the world, the role of social identities in collective actions has become the center of academic attention. Some scholars have claimed that interpersonal or individual reasons have become more important than collective identifications in participating digitally-mediated collective actions. To answer the question that whether social identification has lost its centrality in collective actions in the Internet age, we conducted a meta-analysis of 46 studies (N = 18,242) which examined digitally-mediated collective actions across the world reported between January 2011 and January 2020. We focused on the relationship between social identification and collective action, and the possible moderator effects of group type to be identified (emergent vs. pre-existing group), participation type (actual behavior vs. intention), and WEIRDness of the sample. The analyses showed a moderate to strong relationship between social identification and participation in digitally-mediated collective actions, while group type was the only significant moderator. Accordingly, the relationship between identification with emergent groups and collective action participation was much stronger compared to the relationship between identification with pre-existing groups and collective action participation. We discussed the theoretical implications of the results emphasized the basic dynamics of collective actions.


Keywords:
social identification; collective action; connective action; social media; digital platforms
Author biographies

Serap Akfırat

Dokuz Eylül University, İzmir, Turkey

Serap Akfırat is Associate Professor in Social Psychology at Dokuz Eylul University (Izmir, Turkey). Her research focuses on intergroup relations and intragroup processes, specifically in large-scale groups such as nations. In this regard, she is also interested in leadership processes, collective actions, and social change.

Mete Sefa Uysal

Dokuz Eylül University, İzmir, Turkey; Friedrich Schiller University Jena, Jena, Germany

Mete Sefa Uysal is a postdoctoral researcher and lecturer in the Department of Social Psychology at Friedrich-Schiller-Universität Jena, specializing in collective action, intergroup relations, leadership, and social identity formation.

Fatih Bayrak

Başkent University, Ankara, Turkey

Fatih Bayrak (Ph.D. Cand.) is a research assistant at Baskent University and a manager at MINT Lab (moralintuitionslab.com). His research focuses on political psychology, morality, and intergroup relations. His works examine the underpinnings of certain belief systems, such as epistemically suspect beliefs (i.e., conspiracy, paranormal, and pseudoscientific beliefs) and ideological beliefs. He is also studying how different cognitive styles lead individuals to believe fake news.

Tolga Ergiyen

İzmir University of Economics, İzmir, Turkey

Tolga Ergiyen is a PhD candidate at the Dokuz Eylul University and currently working as a research assistant at the Psychology Department at the Izmir University of Economics. His research interests include intergroup relations, intergroup conflict and wellbeing.

Emir Üzümçeker

Dokuz Eylül University, İzmir, Turkey

Emir Üzümçeker is a PhD candidate in and research assistant in Department of Social Psychology, Dokuz Eylül University, İzmir, Turkey. His research interests include intergroup relations, gender and masculinities.

Taylan Yurtbakan

Dokuz Eylül University, İzmir, Turkey; Istanbul Medipol University, Istanbul, Turkey

Taylan Yurtbakan is a PhD candidate at Dokuz Eylul University. He is currently at the Department of Psychology, Istanbul Medipol University, as a research assistant. His research interests mainly include intergroup contact, and prejudice as well as the link between social contact and social change.

Özlem S. Özkan

Dokuz Eylül University, İzmir, ‎Turkey

Özlem Serap Özkan is a Ph.D candidate and research assistant at Dokuz Eylul University, Department of Psychology (Turkey). Her research focuses on intergroup relations and more particularly on group-based emotions. She is specifically interested in formation of group-based emotions and factors that are related to them.

References

Adam-Troian, J., Bonetto, E., & Arciszewski, T. (2021). “We Shall Overcome”: First-person plural pronouns from search volume data predict protest mobilization across the United States. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 12(8), 1476–1485. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550620987672

Ajzen, I. (1985). From intentions to actions: A theory of planned behavior. In J. Kuhl & J. Beckmann (Eds.), Action control, from cognition to behavior (pp. 11–39). Springer-Verlag. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-69746-3_2

*Alberici, A. I., & Milesi, P. (2013). The influence of the internet on the psychosocial predictors of collective action. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology, 23(5), 373–388. https://doi.org/10.1002/casp.2131

*Alberici, A. I., & Milesi, P. (2016). Online discussion, politicized identity, and collective action. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 19(1), 43–59. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430215581430

*Alberici, A. I., & Milesi, P. (2018). Online discussion and the moral pathway to identity politicization and collective action. Europe’s Journal of Psychology, 14(1), 143–158. https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v14i1.1507

Anduiza, E., Cristancho, C., & Sabucedo, J. M. (2014). Mobilization through online social networks: The political protest of the indignados in Spain. Information, Communication & Society, 17(6), 750–764. https://doi.org/10.1080/1369118X.2013.808360

Arnett, J. J. (2016). The neglected 95%: Why American psychology needs to become less American. American Psychologist, 63(7), 602–614. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.63.7.602

*Ayanian, A. H., & Tausch, N. (2016). How risk perception shapes collective action intentions in repressive contexts: A study of Egyptian activists during the 2013 post-coup uprising. British Journal of Social Psychology, 55(4), 700–721. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12164

Becker, J. C., Tausch, N., Spears, R., & Christ, O. (2011). Committed dis(s)idents: Participation in radical collective action fosters disidentification with the broader in-group but enhances political identification. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(8), 1104–1116. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167211407076

Bennett, W. L., Breunig, C., & Givens, T. (2008). Communication and political mobilization: Digital media and the organization of anti-Iraq war demonstrations in the U.S. Political Communication, 25(3), 269–289. https://doi.org/10.1080/10584600802197434

Bennett, W. L., & Segerberg, A. (2012). The logic of connective action: Digital media and the personalization of contentious politics. Information, Communication & Society, 15(5), 739–768. https://doi.org/10.1080/1369118x.2012.670661

Bennett, W. L., Segerberg, A., & Walker, S. (2014). Organization in the crowd: Peer production in large-scale networked protests. Information, Communication & Society, 17(2), 232–260. https://doi.org/10.1080/1369118x.2013.870379

*Besta, T., Jurek, P., Iwanowska, M., & Szostakowski, D. (2019). Multiple group membership and collective actions of opinion-based internet groups: The case of protests against abortion law restriction in Poland. Computers in Human Behavior, 95, 94–100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2019.01.025

*Bilali, R., Godfrey, E. B., & Freel, S. H. (2020). How an election loss leads to a social movement: Reactions to the 2016 U.S. presidential election among liberals predict later collective action and social movement identification. British Journal of Social Psychology, 59(1), 227–247. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12335

Bimber, B., Flanagin, A. J., & Stohl, C. (2005). Reconceptualizing collective action in the contemporary media environment. Communication Theory, 15(4), 365–388. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2885.2005.tb00340.x

Bliuc, A.-M., McGarty, C., Reynolds, K., & Muntele, D. (2007). Opinion‐based group membership as a predictor of commitment to political action. European Journal of Social Psychology, 37(1), 19–32. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.334

Carlisle, J. E., & Patton, R. C. (2013). Is social media changing how we understand political engagement? An analysis of Facebook and the 2008 presidential election. Political Research Quarterly, 66(4), 883–895. https://doi.org/10.1177/1065912913482758

Castells, M. (2015). Networks of outrage and hope: Social movements in the Internet age. John Wiley & Sons. https://doi.org/10.7312/blau17412-091

Chan, M. (2014). Social identity gratifications of social network sites and their impact on collective action participation. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 17(3), 229–235. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajsp.12068

*Chan, M. (2016). Psychological antecedents and motivational models of collective action: Examining the role of perceived effectiveness in political protest participation. Social Movement Studies, 15(3), 305–321. https://doi.org/10.1080/14742837.2015.1096192

*Chan, M. (2017). Media use and the social identity model of collective action: Examining the roles of online alternative news and social media news. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 94(3), 663–681. https://doi.org/10.1177/1077699016638837

*Chan, W., Cattaneo, L. B., Mak, W. W. S., & Lin, W.-Y. (2017). From moment to movement: Empowerment and resilience as a framework for collective action in Hong Kong. American Journal of Community Psychology, 59(1–2), 120–132. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajcp.12130

Chayinska, M., Minescu, A., & McGarty, C. (2019). ‘We fight for a better future for our country’: Understanding the Ukrainian Euromaidan movement as the emergence of a social competition strategy. British Journal of Social Psychology, 58(1), 45–65. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12283

*Cheng, G. H.-L., Chan, D. K.-S., & Yeung, D. Y. (2019). Correlates of acceptance of wealth inequality: A moderated mediation model. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, Article 900. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00900

Cohen, J. (1960). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 20(1), 37–46. https://doi.org/10.1177/001316446002000104

Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), 155–159. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155

Cooper, H., Hedges, L. V., & Valentine, J. C. (Eds.) (2009). The handbook of research synthesis and meta-analysis. Russell Sage Foundation.

*Dalgar, I., Sumer, N., Thomsen, L., & IJzerman, H. (2021). Testing the relational motivational model of collective action [Unpublished manuscript].

*de Zúñiga, H. G., Jung, N., & Valenzuela, S. (2012). Social media use for news and ındividuals’ social capital, civic engagement and political participation. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 17(3), 319–336. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1083-6101.2012.01574.x

*de Zúñiga, H. G., Molyneux, L., & Zheng, P. (2014). Social media, political expression, and political participation: Panel analysis of lagged and concurrent relationships. Journal of Communication, 64(4), 612–634. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcom.12103

Drury, J., & Reicher, S. (1999). The intergroup dynamics of collective empowerment: Substantiating the social identity model of crowd behavior. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 2(4), 381–402. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430299024005

Drury, J., & Reicher, S. (2000). Collective action and psychological change: The emergence of new social identities. British Journal of Social Psychology, 39(4), 579–604. https://doi.org/10.1348/014466600164642

Drury, J., & Reicher, S. (2009). Collective psychological empowerment as a model of social change: Researching crowds and power. Journal of Social Issues, 65(4), 707–725. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.2009.01622.x

Earl, J., & Kimport, K. (2011). Digitally enabled social change: Activism in the internet age. MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9780262015103.001.0001

Ellemers, N. (1993). The influence of socio-structural variables on identity management strategies. European Review of Social Psychology, 4(1), 27–57. https://doi.org/10.1080/14792779343000013

Garrett, R. K. (2006). Protest in an information society: A review of the literature on social movements and new ICTs. Information, Communication & Society, 9(2), 202–224. https://doi.org/10.1080/13691180600630773

*Girerd, L., Ray, F. A., Priolo, D., Codou, O., & Bonnot, V. (2020). “Free” not to engage: Neoliberal ideology and collective action. The case of the Yellow Vest movement. International Review of Social Psychology, 33(1), Article 7. https://doi.org/10.5334/irsp.363

*Gotlieb, M. R., & Thorson, K. (2017). Connected political consumers: Transforming personalized politics among youth into broader repertoires of action. Journal of Youth Studies, 20(8), 1044–1061. https://doi.org/10.1080/13676261.2017.1305101

Greijdanus, H., de Matos Fernandes, C. A., Turner-Zwinkels, F., Honari, A., Roos, C. A., Rosenbusch, H., & Postmes, T. (2020). The psychology of online activism and social movements: Relations between online and offline collective action. Current Opinion in Psychology, 35, 49–54. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2020.03.003

Harlow, S., & Guo, L. (2014). Will the revolution be tweeted or Facebooked? Using digital communication tools in immigrant activism. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 19(3), 463–478. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcc4.12062

Haslam, S. A., Turner, J. C., Oakes, P. J., McGarty, C., & Reynolds, K. J. (1998). The group as a basis for emergent stereotype consensus. European Review of Social Psychology, 8(1), 203–239. https://doi.org/10.1080/14792779643000128

Henrich, J., Boyd, R., Bowles, S., Camerer, C., Fehr, E., Gintis, H., McElreath, R., Alvard, M., Barr, A., Ensminger, J., Smith Henrich, N., Hill, K., Gil-White, F., Gurven, M., Marlove, F. W., Patton, J. Q., & Tracer, D. (2005). “Economic man” in cross-cultural perspective: Behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(6), 795–815. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X05000142

Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010a). The weirdest people in the world?. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 61–83. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0999152X

Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010b). Most people are not WEIRD. Nature, 466(7302), 29–29. https://doi.org/10.1038/466029a

Henrich, J., McElreath, R., Barr, A., Ensminger, J., Barrett, C., Bolyanatz, A., Cardenas, J. C., Gurven, M., Gwako, E., Henrich, N., Lesorogol, C., Marlowe, F., Tracer, D., & Ziker, J. (2006). Costly punishment across human societies. Science, 312(5781), 1767–1770. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1127333

Higgins, J. P. T., & Thompson, S. G. (2002). Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta‐analysis. Statistics in Medicine, 21(11), 1539–1558. https://doi.org/10.1002/sim.1186

Hogg, M. A., & van Knippenberg, D. (2003). Social identity and leadership processes in groups. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 35, pp. 1–52). Academic Press.

*Hsiao, Y., & Yang, Y. (2018). Commitment in the cloud? Social media participation in the sunflower movement. Information, Communication & Society, 21(7), 996–1013. https://doi.org/10.1080/1369118x.2018.1450434

*Ji, Y., Zhou, Y., & Kim, S. (2017). A moderated mediation model of political collective action in Hong Kong: Examining the roles of social media consumption and social identity. Asian Journal of Communication, 27(5), 497–516. https://doi.org/10.1080/01292986.2017.1336777

Jetten, J., Mols, F., & Selvanathan, H. P. (2020). How economic inequality fuels the rise and persistence of the Yellow Vest Movement. International Review of Social Psychology, 33(1), 2. http://doi.org/10.5334/irsp.356

Jost, J. T., Liviatan, I., van der Toorn, J., Ledgerwood, A., Mandisodza, A., & Nosek, B. A. (2012). System justification: A motivational process with implications for social conflict. In E. Kals & J. Maes (Eds.), Justice and conflicts (pp. 315–327). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-19035-3_19

Jost, J. T., Barberá, P., Bonneau, R., Langer, M., Metzger, M., Nagler, J., Sterling, J., & Tucker, J. A. (2018). How social media facilitates political protest: Information, motivation, and social networks. Political Psychology, 39(S1), 85–118. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12478

Kelly, G., & Breinlinger, S. (1995). Attitudes, intentions, and behavior: A study of women’s participation in collective action. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 25(16), 1430–1445. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.1995.tb02625.x

*Kende, A., van Zomeren, M., Ujhelyi, A., & Lantos, N. (2016). The social affirmation use of social media as a motivator of collective action. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 46(8), 453–469. https://doi.org/10.1111/jasp.12375

Khazraee, E., & Novak, A. N. (2018). Digitally mediated protest: Social media affordances for collective identity construction. Social Media + Society, 4(1). https://doi.org/10.1177/2056305118765740

Klandermans, B. (1984). Mobilization and participation: Social-psychological expansions of resource mobilization theory. American Sociological Review, 49(5), 583–600. https://doi.org/10.2307/2095417

Klandermans, B. (1997). The social psychology of protest. Blackwell Publishers.

Klein, O., Spears, R., & Reicher, S. (2007). Social identity performance: Extending the strategic side of SIDE. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11(1), 28–45. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868306294588

Klein, R. A., Vianello, M., Hasselman, F., Adams, B. G., Adams, R. B., Jr., Alper, S., Aveyard, M., Axt, J., Babaloia, M., Bahník, Š., Berkics, M., Bernstein, M. J., Berry, D. R., Bialobrzeska, O., Bocian, K., Brandt, M., Busching, R., Cai, H., Cambier, F., . . . Nosek, B. A. (2018). Many Labs 2: Investigating variation in replicability across sample and setting. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/9654g

Lee, F. L. F., Chen, H.-T., & Chan, M. (2017). Social media use and university students’ participation in a large-scale protest campaign: The case of Hong Kong’s Umbrella Movement. Telematics and Informatics, 34(2), 457–469. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tele.2016.08.005

Leong, C., Pan, S. L., Bahri, S., & Fauzi, A. (2019). Social media empowerment in social movements: Power activation and power accrual in digital activism. European Journal of Information Systems, 28(2), 173–204. https://doi.org/10.1080/0960085x.2018.1512944

*Lu, C., Wan, C., Hui, P. P., & Tong, Y. (2020). In response to cultural threat: Cultural self-awareness on collective movement participation. Journal Of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 51(1), 70–76. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022119888795

*Mahfud, Y., & Adam-Troian, J. (2021). “Macron demission!”: Loss of significance generates violent extremism for the Yellow Vests through feelings of anomia. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 24(1), 108–124. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430219880954

McGarty, C., Bliuc, A.-M., Thomas, E. F., & Bongiorno, R. (2009). Collective action as the material expression of opinion‐based group membership. Journal of Social Issues, 65(4), 839–857. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.2009.01627.x

McGarty, C., Thomas, E. F., Lala, G., Smith, L. G. E., & Bliuc, A.-M. (2014). New technologies, new identities, and the growth of mass opposition in the Arab Spring. Political Psychology, 35(6), 725–740. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12060

*Morgan, G. S., & Chan, W. Y. (2016). Different paths to protest: Predictors of collective action in the Occupy Movement. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 46(10), 565–582. https://doi.org/10.1111/jasp.12386

Morling, B., & Lamoreaux, M. (2008). Measuring culture outside the head: A meta-analysis of individualism-collectivism in cultural products. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 12(3), 199–221. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868308318260

Mummendey, A., Kessler, T., Klink, A., & Mielke, R. (1999). Strategies to cope with negative social identity: Predictions by social identity theory and relative deprivation theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(2), 229–245. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.76.2.229

Nekmat, E., Gower, K. K., Zhou, S., & Metzger, M. (2019). Connective-collective action on social media: Moderated mediation of cognitive elaboration and perceived source credibility on personalness of source. Communication Research, 46(1), 62–87. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650215609676

*Nekmat, E., & Ismail, I. (2019). Issue-based micromobilization on social media: Mediated pathways linking issue involvement and self-network opinion congruity to expressive support. Computers in Human Behavior, 101, 51–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2019.07.008

Norenzayan, A., Smith, E. E., Kim, B. J., & Nisbett, R. E. (2002). Cultural preferences for formal versus intuitive reasoning. Cognitive Science, 26(5), 653–684. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog2605_4

Oakes, P. J., Haslam, S. A., & Turner J. C. (1994). Stereotyping and social reality. Blackwell Publishing.

*Odağ, Ö., Uluğ, Ö. M., & Solak, N. (2016). “Everyday I’m Çapuling”: Identity and collective action through social network sites in the Gezi Park Protests in Turkey. Journal of Media Psychology, 28(3), 148–159. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-1105/a000202

*Park, N., & Yang, A. (2012). Online environmental community members’ intention to participate in environmental activities: An application of the theory of planned behavior in the Chinese context. Computers in Human Behavior, 28(4), 1298–1306. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2012.02.013

Platow, M. J., & van Knippenberg, D. (2001). A social identity analysis of leadership endorsement: The effects of leader ingroup prototypicality and distributive intergroup fairness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(11), 1508–1519. https://doi.org/10.1177/01461672012711011

Postmes, T., & Brunsting, S. (2002). Collective action in the age of the internet: Mass communication and online mobilization. Social Science Computer Review, 20(3), 290–301. https://doi.org/10.1177/089443930202000306

Postmes, T., Spears, R., & Lea, M. (2002). Intergroup differentiation in computer-mediated communication: Effects of depersonalization. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 6(1), 3–16. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2699.6.1.3

Priante, A., Ehrenhard, M. L., van den Broek, T., & Need, A. (2018). Identity and collective action via computer-mediated communication: A review and agenda for future research. New Media & Society, 20(7), 2647–2669. https://doi.org/10.1177/1461444817744783

Radburn, M., & Stott, C. (2019). The social psychological processes of ‘procedural justice’: Concepts, critiques and opportunities. Criminology & Criminal Justice, 19(4), 421–438. https://doi.org/10.1177/1748895818780200

Reicher, S. (1996). ‘The Crowd’ century: Reconciling practical success with theoretical failure. British Journal of Social Psychology, 35(4), 535–553. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309.1996.tb01113.x

Reicher, S. D. (2001). The psychology of crowd dynamics. In M. Hogg & S. Tindale (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of social psychology: Group processes (pp. 175–197). Blackwell Publishing.

Reicher, S., Haslam, S. A., & Hopkins, N. (2005). Social identity and the dynamics of leadership: Leaders and followers as collaborative agents in the transformation of social reality. The Leadership Quarterly, 16(4), 547–568. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2005.06.007

Rhodes, R. E., & Dickau, L. (2012). Experimental evidence for the intention–behavior relationship in the physical activity domain: A meta-analysis. Health Psychology, 31(6), 724–727. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027290

Saab, R., Spears, R., Tausch, N., & Sasse, J. (2016). Predicting aggressive collective action based on the efficacy of peaceful and aggressive actions. European Journal of Social Psychology, 46(5), 529–543. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2193

*Sabucedo, J.-M., Gómez-Román, C., Alzate, M., van Stekelenburg, J., & Klandermans, B. (2017). Comparing protests and demonstrators in times of austerity: Regular and occasional protesters in universalistic and particularistic mobilisations. Social Movement Studies, 16(6), 704–720. https://doi.org/10.1080/14742837.2017.1338940

*Schumann, S., & Klein, O. (2015). Substitute or stepping stone? Assessing the impact of low-threshold online collective actions on offline participation. European Journal of Social Psychology, 45(3), 308–322. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2084

Selvanathan, H. P., & Lickel, B. (2019). Empowerment and threat in response to mass protest shape public support for a social movement and social change: A panel study in the context of the Bersih movement in Malaysia. European Journal of Social Psychology, 49(2), 230–243. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2502

Shadiqi, M. A., Muluk, H., & Milla, M. N. (2018). Palestinian solidarity action: The dynamics of politicized and religious identity patterns among student activists. Makara Human Behavior Studies in Asia, 22(2), 118–128. https://doi.org/10.7454/hubs.asia.1140818

Simon, B., & Klandermans, B. (2001). Politicized collective identity: A social psychological analysis. American Psychologist, 56(4), 319–331. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.56.4.319

Smith, L. G. E., Gavin, J., & Sharp, E. (2015). Social identity formation during the emergence of the Occupy movement. European Journal of Social Psychology, 45(7), 818–832. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2150

Smith, L. G. E., Thomas, E., & McGarty, C. (2015). “We must be the change we want to see in the world”: Integrating norms and identities through social interaction. Political Psychology, 36(5), 543–557. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12180

Smith, L. G. E., McGarty, C., & Thomas, E. F. (2018). After Aylan Kurdi: How tweeting about death, threat, and harm predict increased expressions of solidarity with refugees over time. Psychological Science, 29(4), 623–634. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617741107

*Stewart, A. L., Leach, C. W., Bilali, R., Çelik, A. B., & Cidam, A. (2019). Explaining different orientations to the 2013 Gezi Park demonstrations in Istanbul, Turkey. British Journal of Social Psychology, 58(4), 829–852. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12316

Stott, C., Ball, R., Drury, J., Neville, F., Reicher, S., Boardman, A., & Choudhury, S. (2018). The evolving normative dimensions of ‘riot’: Towards an elaborated social identity explanation. European Journal of Social Psychology, 48(6), 834–849. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2376

Stott, C., & Drury, J. (2000). Crowds, context, and identity: Dynamic categorization process in the “Poll Tax Riot”. Human Relations, 53(2), 247–273. https://doi.org/10.1177/a010563

Stott, C., & Reicher, S. (1998). Crowd action as intergroup process: Introducing the police perspective. European Journal of Social Psychology, 28(4), 509–529. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0992(199807/08)28:4<509::AID-EJSP877>3.0.CO;2-C

Stürmer, S., & Simon, B. (2004). Collective action: Towards a dual-pathway model. European Review of Social Psychology, 15(1), 59–99. https://doi.org/10.1080/10463280340000117

Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In W. G. Austin & S. Worchel (Eds.), The social psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 33–47). Brooks/Cole.

Tang, G., & Lee, F. L. F. (2013). Facebook use and political participation: The impact of exposure to shared political information, connections with public political actors, and network structural heterogeneity. Social Science Computer Review, 31(6), 763–773. https://doi.org/10.1177/0894439313490625

Thomas, E. F., Mavor, K. I., & McGarty, C. (2012). Social identities facilitate and encapsulate action-relevant constructs. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 15(1), 75–88. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430211413619

*Thomas, E. F., McGarty, C., Lala, G., Stuart, A., Hall, L. J., & Goddard, A. (2015). Whatever happened to Kony2012? Understanding a global Internet phenomenon as an emergent social identity. European Journal of Social Psychology, 45(3), 356–367. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2094

Thomas, E. F., McGarty, C., & Mavor, K. I. (2009). Aligning identities, emotions, and beliefs to create commitment to sustainable social and political action. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 13(3), 194–218. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868309341563

*Thomas, E. F., Smith, L. G. E., McGarty, C., Reese, G., Kende, A., Bliuc, A.-M., Curtin, N., & Spears, R. (2019). When and how social movements mobilize action within and across nations to promote solidarity with refugees. European Journal of Social Psychology, 49(2), 213–229. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2380

Tufekci, Z. (2013). “Not This One”: Social movements, the attention economy, and microcelebrity networked activism. American Behavioral Scientist, 57(7), 848–870. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002764213479369

Turner, J. C., & Haslam, S. A. (2001). Social identity, organizations, and leadership. In M. E. Turner (Ed.), Groups at work: Theory and research (pp. 25–65). Lawrence Erlbaum.

Uskul, A., Kitayama, S., & Nisbett, R. (2008). Ecocultural basis of cognition: Farmers and fishermen are more holistic than herders. PNAS, 105(25), 8552-8556. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0803874105

Uysal, M. S., & Akfırat, S. (2021a). The social identity perspective of social media leadership in collective action participation. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1002/casp.2502

*Uysal, M. S., & Akfırat, S. A. (2021b). Formation of emergent identity: Applying the EMSICA into Gezi Park protests. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430220983597

Vaast, E., Safadi, H., Lapointe, L., & Negoita, B. (2017). Social media affordances form connective action: An examination of microblogging use during the Gulf of Mexico Oil Spill. MIS Quarterly, 41(4), 1179–1205. https://doi.org/10.25300/misq/2017/41.4.08

van Zomeren, M. (2013). Four core social-psychological motivations to undertake collective action. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 7(6), 378–388. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12031

van Zomeren, M. (2016). Building a Tower of Babel? Integrating core motivations and features of social structure into the political psychology of political action. Advances in Political Psychology, 37(S1), 87–114. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12322

van Zomeren, M. (2019). Toward a cultural psychology of collective action: Just how “core” are the core motivations for collective action? Journal of Pacific Rim Psychology, 13, Article e15. https://doi.org/10.1017/prp.2019.7

van Zomeren, M., & Louis, W. R. (2017). Culture meets collective action: Exciting synergies and some lessons to learn for the future. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 20(3), 277–284. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430217690238

van Zomeren, M., Postmes, T., & Spears, R. (2008). Toward an integrative social identity model of collective action: A quantitative research synthesis of three socio-psychological perspectives. Psychological Bulletin, 134(4), 504–535. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.4.504

van Zomeren, M., Spears, R., Fischer, A. H., & Leach, C. W. (2004). Put your money where your mouth is! Explaining collective action tendencies through group-based anger and group efficacy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(5), 649–664. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.87.5.649

Vapnarsky, V., Ucan, E. E., Medin, D., Lynch, E., Atran, S., & Sousa, P. (2001). Folkbiology doesn’t come from folkpsychology: Evidence from Yukatek Maya in cross-cultural perspective. Journal of Cognition and Culture 1(1), 3–42. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853701300063561

*Velasquez, A., Montgomery, G., & Hall, J. A. (2019). Ethnic minorities’ social media political use: How ingroup identification, selective exposure, and collective efficacy shape social media political expression. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 24(4), 147–164. https://doi.org/10.1093/jcmc/zmz007

Vestergren, S., Drury, J., & Chiriac, E. H. (2017). The biographical consequences of protest and activism: A systematic review and a new typology. Social Movement Studies, 16(2), 203–221. https://doi.org/10.1080/14742837.2016.1252665

Vestergren, S., Drury, J., & Chiriac, E. H. (2018). How collective action produces psychological change and how that change endures over time: A case study of an environmental campaign. British Journal of Social Psychology, 57(4), 855–877. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12270

Vestergren, S., Drury, J., & Chiriac, E. H. (2019). How participation in collective action changes relationships, behaviours, and beliefs: An interview study of the role of inter- and intragroup processes. Journal of Social and Political Psychology, 7(1), 76–99. https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.v7i1.903

Wahlström, M. (2011). Taking control or losing control? Activist narratives of provocation and collective violence. Social Movement Studies, 10(4), 367–385. https://doi.org/10.1080/14742837.2011.614107

Waxman, S., & Medin, D. L. (2007). Experience and cultural models matter: Placing firm limits on childhood anthropocentrism. Human Development, 50(1), 23–30. http://doi.org/10.1159/000097681

Webb, T. L., & Sheeran, P. (2006). Does changing behavioral intentions engender behavior change? A meta-analysis of the experimental evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 132(2), 249–268. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.249

*Wlodarczyk, A., Basabe, N., Páez, D., & Zumeta, L. (2017). Hope and anger as mediators between collective action frames and participation in collective mobilization: The case of 15-M. Journal of Social and Political Psychology, 5(1), 200–223. https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.v5i1.471

*Wollast, R., Lüders, A., Li, M., Wu, H., Beattie, P., Nugier, A., & Guimond, S. (2021). Understanding the role of social media in collective action: a comparison between the Yellow Vests movement in France and the Anti-ELAB protests in Hong Kong [Unpublished manuscript]. LAPSCO, UCA, France.

Wright, S. C., Taylor, D. M., & Moghaddam, F. M. (1990). Responding to membership in a disadvantaged group: From acceptance to collective protest. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58(6), 994–1003. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.58.6.994

*Zhou, Q., & Qiu, H. (2020). Predicting online feminist engagement after MeToo: A study combining resource mobilization and integrative social identity paradigms. Chinese Journal of Communication, 13(4), 351–369. https://doi.org/10.1080/17544750.2020.1725077

*= Articles included in the meta-analysis

Additional information

First submission received:
January 19, 2021

Revisions received:
June 28, 2021
September 4, 2021

Accepted for publication:
November 11, 2021

Editor in charge:
David Smahel

Full text

Introduction

The role of digital technologies in collective actions has been drawing attention from a wide range of disciplines such as sociology, political science, communication, and psychology (e.g., Carlisle & Patton, 2013; Castells, 2015; Harlow & Guo, 2014; Postmes et al., 2002; Smith et al., 2018). Especially with the emergence of the digitally-mediated large-scale protests across the world (e.g., Arab Spring in Tunisia and Egypt, Occupy Movements in the US, Gezi Park Protests in Turkey, and Yellow Vest in France), studies on the relationship between digital communication and collective action have increased. Some scholars suggested that collective actions have been changed since organizing social movements have become easier, faster, and cheaper in the Internet age than in the previous times (e.g., Bennett et al., 2008; Garrett, 2006). From this perspective, collective actions organized through social media or other digital platforms are qualitatively different from the traditional ones, because their logic is different and they have become individualized. Therefore, collective actions in the Internet age do not work based on shared social identities, they do not need organizing institutions or leaders any longer (Bennett et al., 2008; Bennett & Segerberg, 2012; Earl & Kimport, 2011). On the other hand, scholars mostly from the social identity approach view collective actions that whether or not organized through social media, all share the basic social psychological processes whereby such factors as social identity, collective efficacy, and perceived injustices interplay (e.g., Postmes et al., 2002; Smith, Gavin, et al., 2015; Thomas et al., 2015; Thomas et al., 2019). Collective action requires the social categorization process and there should be an “us” and the “them” (Drury & Reicher, 1999; Reicher, 1996). Thus, according to the social identity approach, social identification is considered as lying at the heart of all kinds of collective actions.

In the current research, we provide the results of a meta-analysis of the studies on the relationship between social identification and collective action participation in the Internet age, which were published between January 2011 and January 2020. We aim to contribute to the debate of whether collective actions in the Internet age differ qualitatively from the conventional ones. We focus on clarifying the role of social identification in the participation of digitally mediated collective actions. By digitally-mediated collective actions, we refer to the collective actions that mainly utilize cyberspace opportunities such as social media, blogs, and secured chatting systems or applications, which provide easy and fast communication, organization, and mobilization, and thus differ from conventional (pre-digital) actions organized by formal groups or institutions. We also examined the possible moderator effects of group type (emergent vs. pre-existing group), participation type (behavior vs. intention), and WEIRDness (Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, Democratic; see Henrich et al., 2010a) of sample (WEIRD vs. non-WEIRD) on the relationship between social identification and collective action participation.

Social Psychological Accounts of Collective Actions

In social psychology, collective action is defined as the joint conduct of group members who aimed to improve the position of their own group or to reach a common group goal (Wright et al., 1990). The well-known approaches of collective action are based on the findings that social identification is either the strongest predictor of collective action participation or play an essential role in mobilization and participation (e.g., Drury & Reicher, 2009; Jost et al., 2012; Klandermans, 1997; Priante et al., 2018; Stürmer & Simon, 2004; van Zomeren et al., 2008). For instance, The Elaborated Social Identity Model of Crowd Behavior (ESIM; Drury & Reicher, 1999, 2000; Reicher, 1996) posits that collective actions are fundamentally group phenomenon and characteristically intergroup encounters because only shared self-categorization provides definitions of appropriate and possible conduct, and in turn, enable people to act collectively.

Studies, mostly from the social identity approach, showed that to act collectively group members should develop a shared understanding on that their group is illegitimately or unfairly treated (i.e., oppressed, disadvantaged, or deprived) (Ellemers, 1993; Mummendey et al., 1999; Tajfel & Turner, 1986; van Zomeren et al., 2008) which give rise group-based anger, moral outrage, or resentment (Thomas et al., 2009; van Zomeren et al., 2004). Besides, group members should also perceive that they can do something as a group to change the situation, which is mostly conceptualized as collective efficacy (Thomas et al., 2012; van Zomeren, 2013). Developing such a common understanding on these two factors that make collective actions possible would be the result of the interaction between intergroup context (e.g., unfair treatment of outgroup) and intragroup processes (e.g., consensualization process) (Drury & Reicher, 2000; Haslam et al., 1998; Reicher et al., 2005; Vestergren et al., 2018, 2019). Therefore, according to this theoretical perspective, collective actions are characteristically group-level phenomena that encompass both intra- and intergroup processes (Drury & Reicher, 2000; Reicher, 2001; Stott & Reicher, 1998).

Furthermore, research reveals that the most influential individuals that shape common understandings are the most prototypical group members or group representatives (Hogg & van Knippenberg, 2003; Platow & van Knippenberg, 2001; Turner & Haslam, 2001), which indicates that the shared understandings on the intergroup context (e.g., “we are illegitimately treated”) and intragroup factors (e.g., “we should do something”) might be reached through a leadership process. In the collective actions previous to the widespread use of digital technologies, this leading charge was usually, but not necessarily, on to the shoulders of the leaders of formal organizations such as political parties or labor unions (Klandermans, 1984, 1997). In other words, in the process of collective mobilization, the role of framing or shaping the understandings of ingroup members was played by the conventional organizations and their leaders (Bimber et al., 2005).

The Transformed Version of Collective Actions: Connective Actions

Digital technologies, as a new tool of communication, organization, and mobilization have transformed collective actions (Bennett & Segerberg, 2012; Postmes et al., 2002; Thomas et al., 2015). Some scholars proposed that collective actions have undergone a qualitative transformation especially due to the widespread use of digital technologies (e.g., social media) that allow for user-generated content, and make possible for individuals to comment, share, and circulate the contents (e.g., Bennett et al., 2008; Leong et al., 2019; Vaast et al., 2017). Accordingly, digital technologies have a huge impact on collective actions via making mass communication possible and the basic characteristic of collective actions, being a group-level phenomenon, has transformed into a new form (Bennett & Segerberg, 2012). This new form, connective action, has a distinct logic because it presumably operates at interpersonal or even at an individual level rather than working based on shared social identities (Bennett et al., 2008; Bennett & Segerberg, 2012; Earl & Kimport, 2011).

Bennett and colleagues (2008) asserted that compared to conventional social movements, connective action is far more personalized, and its underlying psychological mechanism does not require the symbolic construction of a united us. From this point of view, the connective action participants do not need to develop common understandings or to have a shared ideological frame to make connections (Bennett & Segerberg, 2012). These digital connections are made based on interpersonal relations such as friendship or family. Each participator in digitally-mediated collective actions has its own reasons or concerns for attending, simply because each has a unique combination of individual goals, motives, and interpersonal style. Bennett and Segerberg (2012) claimed that these individualized orientations result in engagement with politics as an expression of personal hopes, lifestyles, and grievances. Individuals reinterpret grievances and re-create their personal meanings in their social media networks. In line with the conceptualization of digitally-mediated collective actions as a personalized mode of actions, Bennett et al. (2014) suggested that social media serve as stitching mechanisms that connect different networks into the coherent organization in the absence of recognized leaders, common goals, or conventional organization.

In a study on Umbrella Movement, the Hong Kong democracy protests, Lee et al. (2017) stated that people engaged in both the collective action of public space occupation and a diverse range of personalized or small-group based actions (e.g., public art creation, attending “civic seminars”, and participating in frontline actions such as setting up road blockades). Nekmat et al. (2019) also showed that receiving messages from personal networks (e.g., friends) are stronger predictors of willingness to participate in collective activities on social media than receiving messages from impersonal ones (e.g., organizational sources). Bennett and colleagues (2014) approach the findings of research on the Arab Spring (e.g., Tufekci, 2013) and Umbrella movements (e.g., Tang & Lee, 2013) as evidence for their arguments. Accordingly, these findings indicate that individuals participated in collective actions through social media, because social media had given people the opportunity to directly connect with political actors or opinion leaders or “networked microcelebrities” (for discussion see Uysal & Akfırat, 2021a). Anduiza et al. (2014) also showed that the main mobilization channels were personal contacts and online social networks rather than the members of formal organizations or broadcast media in the 15-M movements in Spain. Therefore, they concluded that the 15-M movement significantly differed from the traditional collective actions in terms of the characteristics of staging organizations (recently created, without formal membership and scarce resources).

To summarize, the logic of connective action differs significantly from the logic of traditional collective action which requires shared understandings and common group goals. Now, digitally-mediated collective action operates at the interpersonal level rather than group-level. The individual participators mostly have their unique combination of individual motives for participation. Following the logic of connective actions, one can infer that the relationship between social identification and collective action participation has weakened in the Internet age; and social identification might even have lost its predictive power of collective action participation, especially if those actions are mediated by digital platforms.

The Current Study

In the present study, we addressed the role of social identification in collective action participation in the Internet age. We conducted a meta-analysis of the recent studies from 2011 to 2020 on digitally-mediated collective actions (e.g., Arab Spring, Occupy Wall Street, Hong Kong, Yellow Vest, and Gezi Park protests). In the previous studies, group identification appeared as the strongest predictor of collective actions (for a meta-analytic review see van Zomeren et al., 2008), including those which were organized through digital platforms (for a systematic review see Priante et al., 2018). If the assumption that social identification has lost its crucial role in collective action is valid, we should expect that social identification would have a small effect size in predicting participation in collective actions organized through digital technologies (e.g., Facebook and Twitter).

While group identification was found as the strongest antecedent of collective actions in previous studies (Priante et al., 2018; van Zomeren et al., 2008), the increased use of digital platforms that gave rise to questions concerning the mechanisms of collective actions led to a need for the current study. Besides, studies on this topic are diverse with contributions from multiple disciplines that use various theoretical and methodological approaches. Such heterogeneity requires to be addressed by a meta-analytic study which enables us to determine a) the degree of variability of the effects of different studies and b) if there would be considerable variations, how much of these variations could be credited to which factors, i.e., moderators (see Cooper et al., 2009). Therefore, we also aimed to test the effects of possible moderators: group type, participation type, and WEIRDness of the sample.

Group Type

The concept of social identification and the modes of identity are central to the discussions on the similarities and the differences between conventional ways of participating in collective actions and mobilization through digital tools (Alberici & Milesi, 2016, 2018; M. Chan, 2014, 2017). It has been suggested for a long time that the different modes of social identities might affect participation in collective action (Kelly & Breinlinger, 1995; Klein et al., 2007; Simon & Klandermans, 2001; Stürmer & Simon, 2004). Social identification may emerge from the appraisal of injustice and group efficacy beliefs specific to the collective action context (Thomas et al., 2015). Participants’ shared desire for social change provides the base for forming a new ingroup and developing a corresponding social identity different from the previous identifications (Smith, Gavin, et al., 2015; Smith, Thomas, et al., 2015). In a recent systematical review, Vestergren et al. (2017) also identified different forms of psychological change that occur during or after the protests. Accordingly, identities become more politicized and radicalized, and commitment to the action increases (see Drury & Reicher, 2000; Stott & Drury, 2000). Indeed, van Zomeren and colleagues (2008) showed that identification with politicized identity such as action groups had a stronger effect size than identification with the pre-existing groups (r = .43 vs. r = .34). In addition, there are also a number of studies showing identification with emergent groups are strong predictor of participation to digitally mediated collective action (e.g., Chayinska et al., 2019; Odağ et al., 2016; Smith, Gavin et al., 2015; Uysal & Akfırat, 2021b). It seems important to take the aforementioned findings of van Zomeren and colleagues (2008) one step further, and to examine whether identification with an emergent group (e.g., protestor groups, opinion-based groups) better predicts participation to collective action comparing to the identification with pre-existing social groups (e.g., nations, religious groups, ideological groups, etc.). In the present study, we define pre-existing identities as the social identities that come from belongingness to various social groups including political parties, ethnic groups, trade unions, particular ideological groups, etc. that individuals have long before that specific collective action takes place (e.g., W. Chan et al., 2017; Cheng et al., 2019; Mahfud & Adam-Troian, 2021; Sabucedo et al., 2017). On the other hand, we describe emergent identities as action-specific identities, which, either transformed from the preexisting identities into contextual ones or do not exist before the process of that specific collective action starts (e.g., Besta et al., 2019; Bilali et al., 2020; Morgan & Chan, 2016; Uysal & Akfırat, 2021b; Wlodarcyzk et al., 2017). Therefore, the distinction between emergent identities versus pre-existing identities is based on the two dimensions: 1) contextual specificity of the group, emergent groups are those that arose to address a specific problem/issue; and 2) the temporal dimension, the emerging groups do not exist before the collective action process begins. Consequently, a key defining criterion of the emergent identities is that they are contextually specific identities that ‘come to be’, and that could not have been said to exist beforehand. Based on previous findings, we expect that identification with emergent groups would have a higher effect size in predicting collective action participation comparing to the identification with pre-existing groups.

Participation Type

While some of the collective action studies measured actual participation behavior (e.g., W. Chan et al., 2017; Thomas et al., 2015), the others just focused on the intention of participation (e.g., Alberici & Milesi, 2013; Morgan & Chan, 2016). The theory of planned behavior (Ajzen, 1985) asserts that intention is a robust predictor of human behavior, which has been evidenced by numerous studies regarding a wide range of conducts including collective action (Kelly & Breinlinger, 1995; Park & Yang, 2012). Despite some findings supporting the notion that intentions and behaviors are closely linked, the results of a meta-analysis (Webb & Sheeran, 2006) showed that a large change in intention resulted in a small change in actual behavior. Likewise, another meta-analysis by Rhodes and Dickau (2012) demonstrated that changes in intention did not result in a significant change in behavior. Therefore, we thought that the strength of the relationship between social identification and collective action might vary depending on participation type (i.e., behavior vs. intention).

WEIRDness

The vast majority of research in psychology has been carried out with WEIRD (western, educated, industrialized, rich, and democratic) samples (Henrich et al., 2010a). In these studies, it is assumed that human populations have a very small variation and these samples represent the universe (Henrich et al., 2010b). However, the WEIRD samples represent only a small part of the real world (Arnett, 2016), and findings on topics such as self-concepts (e.g., Morling & Lamoreaux, 2008), reasoning styles (e.g., Norenzayan et al., 2002; Uskul et al., 2008), categorization (e.g., Vapnarsky et al., 2001; Waxman & Medin, 2007), cooperation, and fairness (e.g., Henrich et al., 2005, 2006) might not be valid in different macro-contexts (Henrich et al., 2010b). Collective actions have a substantial variety that they occurred in different contexts in many different regions and cultures of the world (see van Zomeren et al., 2008). Whereas numerous studies focused on the relationship between social identification and collective action in underrepresented and non-WEIRD societies, to the knowledge of authors there is no study that examines whether the macro-context in which the collective actions take place moderates this relationship. In this vein, we argue that the WEIRDness of samples should be taken into account to better understand the relationship between social identification and collective action.

To summarize, in the present meta-analysis, we aimed to examine the relationship between social identification and collective action participation in the Internet age. Given the fact that the studies included in the meta-analysis are broad and diverse in terms of context, methodology, and sample, we also expect a considerable amount of heterogeneity. Therefore, we also attempt to specify how much of the assumed heterogeneity could be attributed to the group type, participation type, and WEIRDness of the sample.

Method

Inclusion Criteria and Literature Search

To choose the studies that should be included in the meta-analysis, we used the three following criteria. Our first criterion was that the studies should have included the measures of both social identification and participation in collective action variables. Some studies focusing on the relationship between shared social identity and collective action that took place between 2011 and 2020, were not included in this study, since they were either qualitative (e.g., Khazraee & Novak, 2018), or not digitally mediated (see Selvanathan & Lickel, 2019; Shadiqi et al., 2018).

Second, to make sure the collective action examined in the study was digitally mediated, we chose the studies (1) that were on the large scale collective actions which are known to be overwhelmingly mobilized through digital platforms (e.g., Occupy Movements, Gezi Park Protests, Arab Spring, and Yellow Vest) or (2) that the authors stated the examined collective action was digitally mediated. For example, we included three Yellow Vest studies (i.e., Girerd et al., 2020; Mahfud & Adam-Troian, 2021; Wollast et al., 2021) overwhelmingly known to be organized through digital platforms. The Yellow Vest protests, which were held by thousands of people, were noted as one of the biggest mass mobilizations in Europe, in which digital communication on cyberspace played a central role. In 2018, people, who did not have a common ingroup identity, took to the streets against President Macron in response to the rising inflation and the government’s unequal tax policies in France (Jetten et al., 2020). The initiation and coordination of the protests were carried out through social media networks such as Facebook, instead of traditional formal organizations such as trade unions (Adam-Troian et al., 2021). We also included the studies that the authors stated the examined collective action was digitally mediated even if it has not received worldwide attention (e.g., Besta et al., 2019; Nekmat & Ismail, 2019; Schumann & Klein, 2015). For example, Nekmat and Ismail (2019) focused on ideologically opposing groups on social media and examined expressive support for and against the LGBTQ issue on Facebook as collective action participation. Since participants involved in a collective action that organized entirely through social media, and the authors defined it as a collective action based on micro-mobilization on social media, we included this study in the meta-analysis.

Third, we included the studies that contain Pearson’s r as an indicator of the relationship between social identification and collective action participation. When studies did not provide the correlations upon which analyses were based (i.e., because they only reported standardized or unstandardized coefficients), we contacted the author(s) and asked them to provide the necessary statistics.1

Figure 1. Selection Process of Included Publications.

For the relevant studies, we performed a comprehensive search in the database Web of Science (WoS). The starting-date and end-date we entered to the search engine were January 2011 and January 2020, respectively. In this search, we used the related search terms and their variations in the title, abstracts, and keywords of the articles.2 We reached 1894 articles as a result of the WoS search. Then we refined the results by limiting the WoS categories. Because a huge range of categories are presented by the database from engineering to medicine, we made sure that only the directly relevant categories were included in our search such as communication, political science, political psychology, social psychology, and so on in this phase. After limiting the categories, we got 758 articles, and read the abstract of each. We removed 603 articles based on abstract reading and reached 155 relevant articles as a result. Then, we examined the full texts and decided to include 23 papers which met our including criteria. After that, we searched the reference lists of these 23 papers and found 8 more relevant papers. We also made a call for unpublished or nearly published studies via social media and mail groups, and the authors sent 3 unpublished papers to us.3 Finally, we included 46 studies with different samples published in 34 papers between 2011 and 2020. The whole research plan and inclusion criteria are presented in the diagram (see Figure 1). All data are accessible at https://osf.io/tf5zp/?view_only=e3c13743d63a4d5b889d369b3d5dc0dc

Data Coding

The data from each study were extracted and each author coded the sample sizes and effect sizes. The discrepancies between the authors were checked and resolved through consensus. Basic sample descriptors included sample size (N), mean age, the standard deviation of age, percentage male/female in the sample, and country of the sample (see Table 1).

Table 1. Descriptive Information of All Samples.

Label

N

Female (%)

Male (%)

Age (SD)

Country

Alberici2013-1

71

25.35

74.65

38.2 (11.2)

Italy

Alberici2013-2

147

56.46

43.54

32.5 (13.4)

Italy

Alberici2016-2

192

100

0

47.11 (12.2)

Italy

Alberici2018

143

37.06

62.94

49.2 (12.5)

Italy

Ayanian2016-1,2

146

32.19

67.81

26.20 (-)

Egypt

Besta2019-1

181

59.67

40.33

25.1 (8.90)

Poland

Besta2019-2

262

55.73

44.27

25.6 (6.70)

Poland

Bilali2020

913

60.8

39.2

38.87 (12.12)

US

M. Chan2016

816

52

48

-

Hong Kong

M. Chan2017

818

51.7

48.3

-

Hong Kong

W. Chan2017

589

68.9

31.1

20.8 (3.03)

Hong Kong

Cheng2019

866

48

52

-

Hong Kong

Dalgar2021*

641

55.07

33.23

28.49 (0.74)

Turkey

Girerd2020-1

489

61.65

36.91

41.95 (16.24)

France

Girerd2020-2

201

54.2

45.8

37.61 (14.7)

France

Gotlieb2017

651

49.8

50.2

24.24 (3.27)

US

Hsiao2018

801

48.81

49.81

23.59 (3.91)

Taiwan

Ji2017

639

61.3

38.7

22 (0.69)

Hong Kong

Kende2016-1

148

52

48

29.3 (12.4)

Hungary

Kende2016-2

261

78.2

21.8

21.34 (3.14)

Hungary

Lu2020

189

70.37

29.63

21.26 (2.56)

Hong Kong

Mahfud2021-1

776

50.39

28.48

32.02 (15.19)

France

Mahfud2021-2

511

89.6

9.1

19.39 (3.04)

France

Morgan2016

193

53

47

32.41 (14.00)

US

NekmatIsmail2019

684

54

46

27.1 (6.18)

Singapore

Odag2016

1127

45.87

44.81

30.11 (9.72)

Turkey

Park2012

211

49.8

47.9

25.37 (6.39)

China

Sabucedo2017

285

43.4

56.6

40 (14.49)

Spain

Schumann2015-1

76

80.3

19.7

20.96 (3.93)

Belgium

Schumann2015-2

59

84.5

15.5

19.48 (2.33)

Belgium

Stewart2019-2

327

48.32

38.84

32.38 (13.79)

Turkey

Thomas2015

299

51.37

48.63

20.73 (4.68)

Australia

Thomas2019-1

267

78.7

21.3

21.22 (2.48)

Hungary

Thomas2019-2

190

51.1

48.9

27.51 (9.55)

Germany

Thomas2019-3

159

82.4

17.6

18.5 (1.48)

UK

Thomas2019-4

244

69.7

30.3

40.08 (21.97)

US

Thomas2019-5

344

23.3

76.7

36.50 (15.07)

Australia

Thomas2019-6

163

76.7

23.3

37.40 (10.80)

Romania

Uysal2021

345

62.32

37.68

29.30 (8.72)

Turkey

Velasquez2019

503

50.1

49.9

38.61 (14.28)

US

Wlodarczyk2017

638

56.6

43.4

31.66 (11.39)

Spain

Wollast2021*

169

58.58

40.24

25.81 (9.03)

Hong Kong

Zhou2020

721

100

0

26.90 (-)

China

deZuniga2012

475

67

33

45.76 (12.45)

US

deZuniga2014

312

67

33

49.32 (12.25

US

Note. * = Unpublished paper.

Then, 3 pairs of authors coded each of the moderator variables (group type, participation type, and WEIRDness) independently. That is, each moderator variable was coded by 2 different coders. We calculated Cohen’s (1960) kappa to measure the level of inter-rater agreement for each moderator. Firstly, the two authors coded the group type of each study (i.e., whether the social group was an emergent group specific to the action or pre-existing groups such as gender, race, or political party). The Cohen’s k for group type was .88, which indicates a high percentage of agreement between coders. Disagreements on this moderator stemmed from the three papers that provided the Pearson r’s for both preexisting group identification and emergent group identification (i.e., Bilali et al., 2020; Kende et al., 2016, Study 2; Thomas et al., 2015). We consensually categorized studies into either pre-existing groups or emergent groups by re-examining the collective action context and the measurement tools of each study. Precisely, taking into account the theoretical and empirical considerations, we placed the study by Bilali et al. (2020) and the study by Thomas et al. (2015) into “emergent group” category, while we put the study by Kende et al. (2016) into “preexisting group” category (see supplementary file for detailed information regarding the decision making processes for these three cases). Secondly, the other two authors coded participation type (i.e., whether people participated in an actual collective action or reported their intention to participate) for each study. The Cohen’s k for participation type was .89, which suggests a high inter-rater agreement. Thirdly, the other two authors coded the WEIRDness of the sample for each study (see Table 2), which resulted in a complete agreement.4 Where disagreements regarding the moderators were there, all of the authors re-checked the studies individually, and consensus was reached through discussion.

Table 2. List of Non-WEIRD and WEIRD Countries.

Non-WEIRD Countries (N of study)

 

WEIRD Countries (N of study)

China (2), Egypt (2), Hong Kong (7), Romania (1), Taiwan (1), and Turkey (4).

 

Australia (2), Belgium (2), France (4), Germany (1), Hungary (3), Italy (4), Poland (2), Singapore (1), Spain (2), UK (1), and US (7).

Analysis

We conducted all of the analyses with jamovi software (version 1.6.23, 2021) using the MAJOR package (version 1.2.0). We transformed correlation coefficients into Fisher Z correlation coefficients for analysis and back-transformed for results. This transformation allowed us to consider the sample size. We interpreted results by Cohen’s (1992) guidelines which pointed small (r = .10), medium (r = .30) and large (r = .50) effects. In accordance with the wide range of sample populations, we used the random-effects model which assumes that effects sizes can vary across studies. We analyzed effect sizes obtained through the reported correlation coefficients between social identification and collective action participation. For studies that reported multiple correlations, for example between two subscales of social identity and collective action participation, we took the average of all relevant correlations (Cooper et al., 2009).5

Results

Descriptive Characteristics

Across the 46 studies, the total number of participants was 18.242, of which 59.40% were female. The mean age of the participants was 30.50 (SD = 8.72) years.

Overall Effect Size

We included the 34 studies (with 46 different samples) in the current meta-analysis. The average correlation between social identification and participation to collective action is rz = .48 (95% CI [.416, .549], Z = 14.2, p < .001, see Figure 2). Results revealed that there is a moderate to strong (Cohen, 1992) positive relationship between social identification and participation in collective action. That is, higher identification is associated with frequent participation in collective action. A Cochrac’s Q test was conducted to examine heterogeneity. Results indicated that there is significant heterogeneity across studies (Q (45) = 957.645, p < .001, see Figure 2 and Figure 3). Besides, according to Higgins and Thompson (2002), I2 is a useful and strong indicator of heterogeneity. In the present study, results (I2 = 95.01%) indicate that the substantial variance depends on the variability in true effects size rather than depending on sampling error. Therefore, it can be concluded that there are some potential moderators to explain the amount of heterogeneity.

Figure 2. Forest Graph of Average Weighted Effect Size of Social Identification on Participation to Collective Action and the Effect Sizes
and Confidence Intervals
.

Assessing Publication Bias

We announced to retrieve as many unpublished studies as possible. However, there is a limited amount of unpublished studies (n = 3) included in the meta-analysis. First, according to visual inspection, there is a symmetric distribution which is an indicator of no publication bias (see Figure 3). Moreover, Egger’s test provides substantial evidence. Funnel plot asymmetry was not significant for the association between social identification and participation to collective action (z = 1.17, p = .244). Therefore, regarding evidence, we concluded that there is no sign of publication bias.

Moderation Analysis

The results indicating heterogeneity suggested that there are substantial moderators. Therefore, we conducted a subgroup analysis for the potential three moderators (group type, participation type, and WEIRDness). Results showed that the group type (emergent vs. pre-existing group) significantly moderates the relationship between social identification and participation in collective action, indicating that the relationship was stronger for emergent group identification (rz = .55, 95% CI [.478, .624]) than pre-existing group identification (rz = .34, 95% CI [.228, .450]). All other moderators were not significant (ps > .05). The results were presented in Table 3.

Figure 3. Funnel Plot for the Effect of Social Identification and Participation
to Collective Action
.

Table 3. Summary of the Moderators on the Relationships Between Social Identification and Participation to Collective Action.

Moderator

rz

95% CI

k

p

Total

.48*

[.416, .549]

46

 

Group Type

 

 

 

.001

Group Identity

.34*

[.228, .450]

15

 

Protestor Identity

.55*

[.478, .624]

31

 

Action Type

 

 

 

.130

Intention

.53*

[.442, .614]

26

 

Participation

.43*

[.325, .528]

20

 

Weirdness

 

 

 

.670

Weird

.49*

[.405, .583]

29

 

Non-Weird

.46*

[.363, .563]

17

 

Note. CI, confidence interval; k, number of studies; p, p values from moderation analysis *p < .001.

Discussion

In the present study, we aimed to examine the relationship between social identification and collective action participation through a meta-analysis, and thus, to contribute to the literature regarding the debate on the underlying mechanisms of collective actions in the Internet age (i.e., whether collective actions still do work on the basis of shared social identities). The results showed that there was a moderate to a strong relationship between social identification and participation in the digitally-mediated collective actions. In addition, we examined the role of some potential moderators (i.e., group types to be identified, participation type in collective action, and WEIRDness of the sample). The relationship between collective action participation and identification with emergent groups was found stronger compared to the relationship between collective action participation and identification with pre-existing social groups, while none of the other moderators had a significant effect. These results highlighted importance of the two points regarding the research question of whether underlying social-psychological mechanisms of collective actions in the Internet age are the same as those previously.

First, the current meta-analysis indicated that the studies appear as providing empirical support for collective action, rather than the connective action which is basically proposed as a substitute for collective action in the Internet age. The concept of connective action was suggested as a new form of collective action that does not work based on shared social identities (Bennett et al., 2008; Earl & Kimport, 2011). Rather, this new form of action has been considered as relying on (inter)personal digital connections, which make the process of collective action individualized. If people have participated in digitally-mediated collective actions based overwhelmingly on their individual or interpersonal connections or motivations, results would have shown a weak relationship between social identification and participation. In contrast, our results revealed that social identification is the strong predictor of collective action participation in the Internet age. In other words, consistent with the social identity perspective, collective actions, including those that are organized through social media, seem to share the basic social psychological dynamics (e.g., Postmes et al., 2002; Stott et al., 2018; Thomas et al., 2015; Uysal & Akfırat, 2021a). This result is also consistent with the previous studies that found social identification was the strongest predictor of collective action participation (e.g., Jost et al., 2012; Klandermans, 1997; Stürmer & Simon, 2004; van Zomeren et al., 2008; for a systematic review see Priante et al., 2018).

Second, we also found that the relationship between collective action participation and identification with emergent groups were stronger than identification with pre-existing groups. This finding highlight that the social identification process of collective action might have transformed to some extent in the Internet age. The nature and formation of social identities in the Internet age have been discussed by the researchers from social identity tradition since the collective actions started to be organized through digital platforms. For example, Postmes and Brunsting (2002) argued that digital platforms endorse the promotion of social identity and the formation of new ingroups by influencing the ability to express behavior and the formation of new social identities by reducing the accountability of users. There are also more recent models drawing attention to the changing nature of social identity in digitally-mediated collective actions such as The Social Identity Model of Collective Action (SIMCA; van Zomeren et al., 2008), The Encapsulated Model of Social Identity in Collective Action (EMSICA; Thomas et al., 2009, 2012), and The Identity-Norm Nexus Formation Model (INN; Smith, Thomas et al., 2015). McGarty et al. (2014) proposed that social movements in the Internet age have involved a process by which people previously share similar ideas come to see themselves as a coherent social group or form opinion-based groups throughout communication on digital platforms, as the intergroup context demands (see also Bliuc et al., 2007; McGarty et al., 2009). Thomas and colleagues (2015) asserted that social media helps the formation of a contextually meaningful social identity and that social identification may emerge from the appraisal of injustice and group efficacy beliefs. Similarly, according to the INN model (Smith, Thomas et al., 2015), social movements are not defined by pre-existing groups, ideologies, or identities in the Internet age anymore. Rather, they are driven by the participants’ shared aspiration for social change which provides the basis for forming the new ingroup and developing a social identity. Despite some differences, the common point of the recent models is that all suggest digitally-mediated collective actions mostly rely on the emergent identities which are typically specific to the action context (e.g., protestor identity).

Considering the finding that collective action participation correlated strongly with emergent group identification than pre-existing group identification, which is also in line with the assumptions of the recent models mentioned above, digital communication appears to help the formation of new ingroups and to facilitate identification with these newly formed context-specific protestor groups. Supporting this point, Adam-Troian et al. (2021) showed that increase in plural pronoun use (e.g., ‘we’, ‘us’) in digital platforms predicted Protest and Participant counts the following week in the US, and the reverse was not true.

Although it might be plausible concluding that forming new groups and identifying with emergent groups become more essential than identifying with pre-existing groups in large scale collective actions organized through digital platforms, one must be cautious when reaching such an inference. Given the fact that the studies in the current meta-analysis were conducted either during the collective actions or just a short while later, it is also possible that people have participated in the collective actions simply because their formal organizations or ideological groups might have demanded it. But afterward, the same people would have started to identify with the emergent group stronger than their pre-existing group. This might have been because of the transformative effect of experiencing the collective action itself (Drury & Reicher, 2000, 2009) that the self-categorizations of crowd members might change as a function of their interactions with the out-group during the action (see Vestergren et al., 2018, 2019), and a social identity that doesn’t exist previously may develop as a function of intergroup dynamics. On the other hand, this situation might not mean a transformation of pre-existing identities into emergent identities. People might have been highly identified with an emergent group just because it becomes salient during the action. That is, being a protestor would be more salient and dominant compared to being, for example, an environmentalist, a socialist, or an LGBTQ+. Whatever the case, the aforementioned transformation seems to occur just after the action started, as a function of intergroup confrontation as ESIM suggested. As such, digital platforms like microblogging and social media would contribute very little, if not any, to this process. 

However, the other possibility also seems very plausible. That is, the influence of digital platforms on social identities might begin long before the collective action takes place. Different from the previous years, today people communicate and share information, ideas, and emotions instantaneously thanks to the widespread use of internet-based platforms (Jost et al., 2018). These online behaviours can cultivate the strong social identity which seems to be required for offline collective action (for a review see Greijdanus et al., 2020). Digital technologies might facilitate consensus process through sharing a) information and ideas on the context that out-group treat the ingroup illegitimately, unfairly, and unjustly, b) the emotions as the appraisal of the unjust outgroup treatment, i.e., resentment, anger, c) the contents of the emergent group that is in the process of being formed, i.e., values and norms d) the route of the action.

Another point we have to note is that neither participation type nor WEIRDness of the sample as the potential moderators that were considered as to explain the heterogeneity was not found significant in the relationship between identification and collective action participation. Regarding the participation type, this result is in line with the theory of planned behavior (Ajzen, 1985), which suggests intention is the strongest predictor of behavior. It should, however, be stated that this result does not mean that intention predicts collective action participation. It just indicates that the effect of social identification is the same for these two types of measures. Nevertheless, it is noteworthy that one’s identification with the relevant group predicts both collective action intentions and behaviors. As for WEIRDness, an increasing number of studies tested the social psychological models of collective action participation which placed the social identification in the heart of the model such as SIMCA in different contexts including non-weird countries (see van Zomeren, 2019). Moreover, numerous studies examined the “core motivations” of collective action (van Zomeren, 2016; van Zomeren et al., 2008) including social identification in non-weird contexts (Ayanian & Tausch, 2016; Saab et al., 2016; Uysal & Akfırat, 2021b; for discussion see van Zomeren & Louis, 2017). The vast majority of these studies provided similar findings with WEIRD samples that social identification is a strong predictor of collective action participation. Yet, van Zomeren (2016, 2019) noted that although core motivations such as social identification might be similar across cultures, how they come into the fore and trigger action is context-sensitive. That is, these core motivations need to be unlocked by cultural and contextual conditions.

The current paper has also some limitations. First, although it is impossible to include qualitative research into the meta-analysis, this impossibility limits our understandings of how digital platforms contribute to the social identification process, and thus collective actions from the initial to the final phase (e.g., Smith et al., 2018). Second, non-WEIRD samples included in the current meta-analysis were from only five different cultures (China, Egypt, Hong Kong, Taiwan, and Turkey). Including more diverse cultural settings as a non-WEIRD might allow us to better comprehend cultural variations in the relationship between social identification and collective action participation via digital platforms. Third, though collective action covers a large range of collective behavior from sign campaign to marches, sit-in actions to physical encounters with the out-groups (e.g., police), we did not differentiate between them. We only make a distinction between actual behavior and behavioral intention. Although there is an assumption in collective action research that participation in all kinds of collective actions shares the same psychological mechanisms, some scholars argue that there are conceptual distinctions among offline collective actions, protests, riots, and uprisings (Radburn & Stott, 2019; Wahlström, 2011). Further studies should focus on the moderator role of the distinctions between the action types.

Lastly, it may not be so easy to separate emerging identities from pre-existing ones. Although our findings indicate that pre-existing group identity is a weaker predictor of participation in collective actions than emergent group identity, especially in the Internet age, this does not mean that emergent or collective action-specific identities are entirely independent of the previous identities. Further research that would examine the dynamic interplay between pre-existing identities and emergent group identities seems to be needed. We also encourage further studies that focus on those who do not have politicized identities before and identify with an emergent identity in collective actions.

Conclusion

Despite the limitations, the present meta-analysis provides support for the notion that social identification is still the basic psychological process of collective action participation in the Internet age, in contrast with the assumptions of connective action. The results revealed that there is a moderate to a strong positive relationship between social identification and participation in collective action. Moreover, our findings showed that group type moderates the relationship between social identification and collective action participation in the digital era, collective action participation has a stronger correlation with emergent group identification than pre-existing group identification.

Footnotes

1 We contacted the authors of 18 studies in which Pearson’s r was not reported. The seven of them responded to us providing the necessary statistics (a response rate of 39%).

2 The list all search terms can be seen at the project page on OSF.

3 The call for unpublished data can be seen at the project page on OSF.

4 We used the same WEIRDness categories as in Many Labs 2 (Klein et al., 2018). As Singapore was not included in Many Labs 2, we decided to categorize Singapore in the WEIRD category by consensus among the authors. In addition, we used the cultural distance tool (Muthukrishna et al., 2020), which was developed based on the World Value Survey, as empirical support for our decision and compared Singapore with the US. It was seen that Singapore has a value of .0419, which is even smaller than most countries treated as a WEIRD such as Poland (.0885), France (.0988), Hungary (.1201), and Spain (.0846). Thus, it is reasonable to categorizing Singapore in the WEIRD countries list. We also re-analyzed data by categorizing Singapore as non-WEIRD, but there were no statistically significant differences in the pattern of results (p = .739).

5 In Thomas et al. (2015) both real participation in collective action and intention were measured by the same scale. Since the items assessing the real behaviors seem to be more concrete evidence, we considered the scale as a measure for real participation rather than a measure for intention.

Supplementary Material

One of the purposes of the current study is to examine the effects of the “Group Type” moderator, in the relationship between identification and collective action participation. This required us to classify each study in terms of the identification groups (i.e., preexisting or emergent). However, the three of the studies provided the correlation coefficients for both the emergent identities and preexisting identities (see Table S1). When trying to solve this issue and to place the study in the correct category, we took into account the theoretical and empirical considerations.

One of these studies that provided the statistics for both identification types was Bilali et al.’s longitudinal study (2020), in which both liberal identification (pre-existing) and movement identification (Anti-Trump) were measured. In this study, political identification was assessed twice (Time 1 and Time 2). However, movement identification and movement participation were assessed once (only at Time 2). None of the correlation coefficients included in the meta-analysis was yielded from the longitudinal data. It is also possible that longitudinal or cross-sectional measures constitute another moderator, which we did not examine. Therefore, as a result of the discussion, we concensualized that movement identification (emergent) should be included in the meta-analysis.

Regarding the study by Kende et al. (2016), we examined the measurement tools that emergent identity was assessed by a single item that “it is important to me that I can talk to other students about issues that concern us”, while preexisting group identification (student identification) was assessed by a frequently used tool with three items (Becker et al., 2011). As a result of discussion we decided to include this in the preexisting identity category.

Regarding the study by Thomas et al. (2015), as global identification was assessed by items like “how strongly do you feel attachment to the world as a whole?”, we agreed that the abstraction level of global categorization is broader than group-level categorization. Turner and Haslam (2001) distinguished the three levels of abstraction: interpersonal (e.g., being smart), intergroup (being Turkish), interspecies (being human). A social identity is usually considered more inclusive categorization than personal identities but less inclusive than interspecies (Oakes et al., 1994). On the other hand the authors also provided correlation coefficients for a social group based identity as well (i.e., Anti-Kony opinion-based social identification). Based on this theoretical distinction we thought that it might be more appropriate to place this study into “emergent identity” category.

Table S1. Classification of the Studies Included in the Meta-Analysis Based on the “Group Type” Moderator.

 

Emergent

Preexisting

1. Alberici & Milesi (2013) Study 1

Politicized identity (Italian Five Star Movement)

 

2. Alberici & Milesi (2013) Study 2

 

Political identification

3. Alberici & Milesi (2016) Study 2

Politicized identity (Italian SNOQ activists)

 

4. Alberici & Milesi (2018)

Politicized identity (Italian Water Movement)

 

5. Ayanian & Tausch (2016) (Two studies)

Politicized identity. (Egypt Anti-Mursi)

 

6. Besta et al. (2019) (Two studies)

Proster identity (movements against Polish anti-abortion law)

 

7. Bilali et al. (2020)

Anti-Trump Movement identification*

Political identification (being leftist-rightist)

8. M. Chan (2016)

 

Political party identification

9. M. Chan (2017)

Identification with HongKong prodemocracy movement

 

10. W. Chan et al. (2017)

 

Hong Konger identity

11. Cheng et al. (2019)

 

Socio-economic status

12. Dalgar et al. (2021)

Gezi Park Protestors identification

 

13. Girerd et al. (2020) (Two studies)

 

Neoliberal identity

14. Gotlieb & Thorson (2017)

 

Green living identification

15. Hsiao & Yang (2018)

Identification with Sunflower Movement in Taiwan.

 

16. Ji et al. (2017)

 

Hong Konger identification

17. Kende et al. (2016) Study 1

Politicized group identification (student occupation of an auditorium),

 

18. Kende et al. (2016) Study 2

Politicized group identification

Grup identification (being student)*

19. Lu et al. (2020)

 

HongKonger,

20. Mahfud et al. (2021) (Two studies)

 

Political identification

21. Morgan & Chan (2016)

Identification with Occupy movement

 

22. Nekmat & Ismail (2019)

 

LGBTQ

23. Odağ et al. (2016)

Gezi Park Çapulcu

 

24. Park & Yang (2012)

 

Identifcation with being environmetalist

25. Sabucedo et al. (2017)

 

Party identification (Europe austerity policies)

26. Schumann & Klein (2015) (Two studies)

 

Identification with being Environmentlist

27. Stewart et al. (2019)

Identification with the protestors (Gezi Park)

 

28. Thomas et al. (2015)

Anti-Kony Opinion-based Social Identification*

Global identification

29. Thomas et al. (2019) (6 different samples)

Self-categorization as a supporter of Syrian refugees

 

31. Uysal & Akfırat (2021b)

Gezi Park Protestor identification

 

32. Velasquez et al. (2019)

 

Hispanic/Latino identification

33. Wlodarczyk et al. (2017)

Protestor group (15 M) identification

 

34. Wollast et al. (2021) (two samples)

Hongkong anti ELAB identification

 

35. Zhou & Qiu (2020)

 

Womenhood, feminist identification

36. de Zúñiga et al. (2012)

 

Political party identification

37. de Zúñiga et al. (2014)

 

Political party identification

Note. The studies, bold in the table, reported the correlation coefficients for both preexisting group identification and emergent group identification.
* The correlation coefficient included in the meta-analysis.

Metrics

2733

Views

1329

PDF views