Mindfulness mediates relations between anxiety with problematic smartphone use severity

Vol.16,No.1(2022)

Abstract

A growing body of literature has demonstrated relations between mood- and anxiety-related psychopathology with problematic smartphone use (PSU) symptom severity. However, there has been little empirical inquiry of potential mediators of these relationships. The current study examined trait mindfulness and smartphone use expectancies as mediators of the relation between depression/anxiety and PSU severity in 352 undergraduate students. Participants completed an online survey that measured depression, anxiety, smartphone use expectancies, and PSU severity. Structural equation modeling demonstrated that trait mindfulness was inversely associated, and smartphone use expectancies were positively associated, with PSU severity. Trait mindfulness significantly mediated relations between anxiety and PSU severity. Results provide implications for understanding PSU within the context of theoretical models of PSU’s development, and highlight the role of mindfulness as an emotion regulation strategy and potential treatment for PSU.


Keywords:
mindfulness; problematic smartphone use; depression; anxiety; structural equation modeling
Author biographies

Caleb J. Hallauer

University of Toledo, Toledo, Ohio, USA

Caleb Hallauer is a graduate student in clinical psychology at the University of Toledo. His research interests include problematic technology use (smartphones, video games, social media, etc.) and antecedent risk/protective factors, such as trauma, depression, anxiety, emotion regulation, and use expectancies.

Emily A. Rooney

University of Toledo, Toledo, Ohio, USA

Emily Rooney is a clinical psychology graduate student at the University of Toledo. Her research interests are trauma-related psychopathology, subsequent risk/protective factors and transdiagnostic mechanisms, specifically emotion regulation.

Joel Billieux

University of Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzerland

Joël Billieux is associate professor of clinical psychology, psychopathology, and psychological assessment at the university of Lausanne, and Research Unit Director of the Cognitive and Affective Regulation Lab (CARLA), Institute of Psychology (IP), University of Lausanne. His main area of research regards the psychological factors (cognitive, affective, motivational, interpersonal) involved in the etiology of addictive behaviors, with a particular focus on self-regulation-related processes. Another focus of his research is the conceptualization and diagnosis of behavioral addictions. More recently, he also conducted research focusing on the effect of emerging technologies on human behavior.

Brian J. Hall

Center for Global Health Equity, NYU Shanghai, Shanghai, China

Brian J. Hall is a Professor of Global Public Health and Director of the Center for Global Health Equity at NYU Shanghai. He is also an Associated Full Professor at the School of Global Public Health, New York University, and an Associate Faculty Member in the Department of Health, Behavior and Society at the Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health. His research agenda utilizes qualitative and quantitative approaches to address diverse population health inequalities (mental health, behavioral addiction, and sexual health) among migrants and minoritized or at-risk communities.

Jon Elhai

University of Toledo, Toledo, Ohio, USA

Jon Elhai is Professor of Psychology and Psychiatry at the University of Toledo. His primary area of research is in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), studying the disorder’s underlying dimensions, and relations with cognitive coping processes and externalizing behaviors. He also has a program of research on cyberpsychology, examining problematic internet and smartphone use. This is the corresponding author for the article (contact@jon-elhai.com).

References

Anderson, E. L., Steen, E., & Stavropoulos, V. (2017). Internet use and Problematic Internet Use: A systematic review of longitudinal research trends in adolescence and emergent adulthood. International Journal of Adolescence and Youth, 22(4), 430–454. https://doi.org/10.1080/02673843.2016.1227716

Andrews, S., Ellis, D. A., Shaw, H., & Piwek, L. (2015). Beyond self-report: Tools to compare estimated and real-world smartphone use. Plos One, 10(10), Article e0139004. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0139004

Arpaci, I. (2021). Relationships between early maladaptive schemas and smartphone addiction: The moderating role of mindfulness. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 19(3), 778–792. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-019-00186-y

Arslan, G. (2017). Psychological maltreatment, forgiveness, mindfulness, and internet addiction among young adults: A study of mediation effect. Computers in Human Behavior, 72, 57–66. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.02.037

Billieux, J. (2012). Problematic use of the mobile phone: A literature review and a pathways model. Current Psychiatry Reviews, 8(4), 299–307. https://doi.org/10.2174/157340012803520522

Billieux, J., Flayelle, M., Rumpf, H.-J., & Stein, D. J. (2019). High involvement versus pathological involvement in video games: A crucial distinction for ensuring the validity and utility of gaming disorder. Current Addiction Reports, 6(3), 323–330. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40429-019-00259-x

Billieux, J., Maurage, P., Lopez-Fernandez, O., Kuss, D. J., & Griffiths, M. D. (2015). Can disordered mobile phone use be considered a behavioral addiction? An update on current evidence and a comprehensive model for future research. Current Addiction Reports, 2(2), 156–162. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40429-015-0054-y

Brand, M., Laier, C., & Young, K. S. (2014). Internet addiction: Coping styles, expectancies, and treatment implications. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, Article 1256. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01256

Brand, M., Wegmann, E., Stark, R., Müller, A., Wölfling, K., Robbins, T. W., & Potenza, M. N. (2019). The Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model for addictive behaviors: Update, generalization to addictive behaviors beyond internet-use disorders, and specification of the process character of addictive behaviors. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 104, 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.06.032

Brand, M., Young, K. S., Laier, C., Wölfling, K., & Potenza, M. N. (2016). Integrating psychological and neurological considerations regarding the development and maintenance of specific internet-use disorders: An interaction of person-affect-cognition-execution (I-PACE) model. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 71, 252–266. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.08.033

Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 822–848. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822

Busch, P. A., & McCarthy, S. (2021). Antecedents and consequences of problematic smartphone use: A systematic literature review of an emerging research area. Computers in Human Behavior, 114, Article 106414. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2020.106414

Canale, N., Moretta, T., Pancani, L., Buodo, G., Vieno, A., Dalmaso, M., & Billieux, J. (2021). A test of the pathway model of problematic smartphone use. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 10(1), 181–193. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.2020.00103

Chen, C., Zhang, K. Z. K., Gong, X., & Lee, M. (2019). Dual mechanisms of reinforcement reward and habit in driving smartphone addiction. Internet Research, 29(6), 1551–1570. https://doi.org/10.1108/INTR-11-2018-0489

Cooper, M. L., Kuntsche, E., Levitt, A., Barber, L. L., & Wolf, S. (2016). Motivational models of substance use: A review of theory and research on motives for using alcohol, marijuana, and tobacco. In K. J. Sher (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of substance use and substance use disorders (pp. 375–421). Oxford University Press.

Csibi, S., Griffiths, M. D., Demetrovics, Z., & Szabo, A. (2021). Analysis of problematic smartphone use across different age groups within the ‘components model of addiction.’ International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 19(3), 616–631. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-019-00095-0

Curran, P. G. (2016). Methods for the detection of carelessly invalid responses in survey data. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 66, 4–19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2015.07.006

Dempsey, A. E., O’Brien, K. D., Tiamiyu, M. F., & Elhai, J. D. (2019). Fear of missing out (FoMO) and rumination mediate relations between social anxiety and problematic Facebook use. Addictive Behaviors Reports, 9, Article 100150. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.abrep.2018.100150

Duke, É., & Montag, C. (2017a). Smartphone addiction and beyond: Initial insights on an emerging research topic and its relationship to Internet addiction. In C. Montag & M. Reuter (Eds.), Internet addiction: Neuroscientific approaches and therapeutical implications including smartphone addiction (pp. 359–372). Springer.

Duke, É., & Montag, C. (2017b). Smartphone addiction, daily interruptions, and self-reported productivity. Addictive Behaviors Reports, 6, 90–95. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.abrep.2017.07.002

Elhai, J. D., Dvorak, R. D., Levine, J. C., & Hall, B. J. (2017). Problematic smartphone use: A conceptual overview and systematic review of relations with anxiety and depression psychopathology. Journal of Affective Disorders, 207, 251–259. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2016.08.030

Elhai, J. D., Levine, J. C., Alghraibeh, A. M., Alafnan, A. A., Aldraiweesh, A. A., & Hall, B. J. (2018d). Fear of missing out: Testing relationships with negative affectivity, online social engagement, and problematic smartphone use. Computers in Human Behavior, 89, 289–298. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2018.08.020

Elhai, J. D., Levine, J. C., & Hall, B. J. (2019a). The relationship between anxiety symptom severity and problematic smartphone use: A review of the literature and conceptual frameworks. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 62, 45–52. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2018.11.005

Elhai, J. D., Levine, J. C., O’Brien, K. D., & Armour, C. (2018b). Distress tolerance and mindfulness mediate relations between depression and anxiety sensitivity with problematic smartphone use. Computers in Human Behavior, 84, 477–484. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2018.03.026

Elhai, J. D., Tiamiyu, M. F., & Weeks, J. W. (2018a). Depression and social anxiety in relation to problematic smartphone use: The prominent role of rumination. Internet Research, 28(2), 315–332. https://doi.org/10.1108/IntR-01-2017-0019

Elhai, J. D., Tiamiyu, M. F., Weeks, J. W., Levine, J. C., Picard, K. J., & Hall, B. J. (2018c). Depression and emotion regulation predict objective smartphone use measured over one week. Personality and Individual Differences, 133, 21–28. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.04.051

Elhai, J. D., Yang, H., Dempsey, A. E., & Montag, C. (2020a). Rumination and negative smartphone use expectancies are associated with greater levels of problematic smartphone use: A latent class analysis. Psychiatry Research, 285, Article 112845. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2020.112845

Elhai, J. D., Yang, H., Fang, J., Bai, X., & Hall, B. (2020b). Depression and anxiety symptoms are related to problematic smartphone use severity in Chinese young adults: Fear of missing out as a mediator. Addictive Behaviors, 101, Article 105962. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2019.04.020

Elhai, J. D., Yang, H., McKay, D., & Asmundson, G. J. G. (2020c). COVID-19 anxiety symptoms associated with problematic smartphone use severity in Chinese adults. Journal of Affective Disorders, 274, 576–582. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2020.05.080

Elhai, J. D., Yang, H., & Montag, C. (2019b). Cognitive- and emotion-related dysfunctional coping processes: Transdiagnostic mechanisms explaining depression and anxiety’s relations with problematic smartphone use. Current Addiction Reports, 6(4), 410–417. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40429-019-00260-4

Fischer-Grote, L., Kothgassner, O. D., & Felnhofer, A. (2019). Risk factors for problematic smartphone use in children and adolescents: A review of existing literature. Neuropsychiatrie, 33(3), 179–190. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40211-019-00319-8

Goldberg, S. B., Tucker, R. P., Greene, P. A., Davidson, R. J., Wampold, B. E., Kearney, D. J., & Simpson, T. L. (2018). Mindfulness-based interventions for psychiatric disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 59, 52–60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2017.10.011

Gomez, R., Stavropoulos, V., & Griffiths, M. D. (2020). Confirmatory factor analysis and exploratory structural equation modelling of the factor structure of the Depression Anxiety and Stress Scales-21. Plos One, 15(6), Article e0233998. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0233998

Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Review of General Psychology, 2(3), 271–299. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271

Harris, B., McCredie, M., & Fields, S. (2020). Examining the psychometric properties of the Smartphone Addiction Scale and its short version for use with emerging adults in the U.S. Computers in Human Behavior Reports, 1, Article 100011. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chbr.2020.100011

Hayes, A. F. (2017). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach (2nd ed.). Guilford Press.

Hedman-Lagerlöf, M., Hedman-Lagerlöf, E., & Öst, L.-G. (2018). The empirical support for mindfulness-based interventions for common psychiatric disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 48(13), 2116–2129. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291718000259

James, R. J. E., & Tunney, R. J. (2017). The need for a behavioural analysis of behavioural addictions. Clinical Psychology Review, 52, 69–76. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2016.11.010

Kardefelt-Winther, D. (2014). A conceptual and methodological critique of internet addiction research: Towards a model of compensatory internet use. Computers in Human Behavior, 31, 351–354. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.10.059

Kardefelt-Winther, D. (2017). Conceptualizing Internet use disorders: Addiction or coping process? Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 71(7), 459–466. https://doi.org/10.1111/pcn.12413

Kardefelt-Winther, D., Heeren, A., Schimmenti, A., van Rooij, A., Maurage, P., Carras, M., Edman, J., Blaszczynski, A., Khazaal, Y., & Billieux, J. (2017). How can we conceptualize behavioural addiction without pathologizing common behaviours? Addiction, 112(10), 1709–1715. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.13763

Kim, H.-J., Min, J.-Y., Kim, H.-J., & Min, K.-B. (2017). Accident risk associated with smartphone addiction: A study on university students in Korea. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 6(4), 699–707. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.6.2017.070

Király, O., Potenza, M. N., Stein, D. J., King, D. L., Hodgins, D. C., Saunders, J. B., Griffiths, M. D., Gjoneska, B., Billieux, J., Brand, M., Abbott, M. W., Chamberlain, S. R., Corazza, O., Burkauskas, J., Sales, C. M. D., Montag, C., Lochner, C., Grünblatt, E., Wegmann, E., … Demetrovics, Z. (2020). Preventing problematic internet use during the COVID-19 pandemic: Consensus guidance. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 100, Article 152180. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2020.152180

Kircaburun, K., Griffiths, M. D., & Billieux, J. (2019). Trait emotional intelligence and problematic online behaviors among adolescents: The mediating role of mindfulness, rumination, and depression. Personality and Individual Differences, 139, 208–213. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2018.11.024

Lachmann, B., Duke, É., Sariyska, R., & Montag, C. (2017). Who’s addicted to the smartphone and/or the Internet? Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 8(3), 182–189. https://doi.org/10.1037/ppm0000172

Lan, Y., Ding, J.-E., Li, W., Li, J., Zhang, Y., Liu, M., & Fu, H. (2018). A pilot study of a group mindfulness-based cognitive-behavioral intervention for smartphone addiction among university students. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 7(4), 1171–1176. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.7.2018.103

Lee, D., Namkoong, K., Lee, J., Lee, B. O., Jung, Y. C. (2019). Lateral orbitofrontal gray matter abnormalities in subjects with problematic smartphone use. Journal of Behavioral Addiction, 8(3), 404–411. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.8.2019.50

Lei, P.-W., & Shiverdecker, L. K. (2020). Performance of estimators for confirmatory factor analysis of ordinal variables with missing data. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 27(4), 584–601. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2019.1680292

Lemenager, T., Hoffmann, S., Dieter, J., Reinhard, I., Mann, K., & Kiefer, F. (2018). The links between healthy, problematic, and addicted internet use regarding comorbidities and self-concept-related characteristics. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 7(1), 31–43. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.7.2018.13

Lewis, J., & Martin, A. (2020). Behavioral dynamics of depression and social anxiety in relation to smartphone addiction. Review of Contemporary Philosophy, 19, 78–84. https://doi.org/10.22381/RCP1920208

Leyland, A., Rowse, G., & Emerson, L.-M. (2019). Experimental effects of mindfulness inductions on self-regulation: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Emotion, 19(1), 108–122. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000425

Leyro, T. M., Zvolensky, M. J., & Bernstein, A. (2010). Distress tolerance and psychopathological symptoms and disorders: A review of the empirical literature among adults. Psychological Bulletin, 136(4), 576–600. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019712

Li, X., Oviedo-Trespalacios, O., Rakotonirainy, A., & Yan, X. (2019). Collision risk management of cognitively distracted drivers in a car-following situation. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 60, 288–298. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2018.10.011

Lopez-Fernandez, O. (2017). Short version of the Smartphone Addiction Scale adapted to Spanish and French: Towards a cross-cultural research in problematic mobile phone use. Addictive Behaviors, 64, 275–280. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2015.11.013

Luberto, C. M., Magidson, J. F., & Blashill, A. J. (2017). A case study of individually delivered mindfulness-based cognitive behavioral therapy for severe health anxiety. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 24(4), 484–495. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cbpra.2016.10.001

Mohsenabadi, H., Shabani, M. J., & Zanjani, Z. (2018). Factor structure and reliability of the Mindfulness Attention Awareness Scale for adolescents and the relationship between mindfulness and anxiety in adolescents. Iranian Journal of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, 13(1), Article e64097. https://doi.org/10.5812/ijpbs.64097

Montag, C. (2019). The neuroscience of smartphone/social media usage and the growing need to include methods from ‘Psychoinformatics’. In F. Davis, R. Riedl, J. vom Brocke, P. M. Léger, & A. Randolph (Eds.), Information Systems and Neuroscience (pp. 275–283). Springer.

Montag, C., Sindermann, C., Becker, B., & Panksepp, J. (2016). An affective neuroscience framework for the molecular study of internet addiction. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, Article 1906. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01906

Montag, C., Wegmann, E., Sariyska, R., Demetrovics, Z., & Brand, M. (2021). How to overcome taxonomical problems in the study of Internet use disorders and what to do with “smartphone addiction”? Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 9(4), 908–914. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.8.2019.59

Montes, K. S., Witkiewitz, K., Pearson, M. R., & Leventhal, A. M. (2019). Alcohol, tobacco, and marijuana expectancies as predictors of substance use initiation in adolescence: A longitudinal examination. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 33(1), 26–34. https://doi.org/10.1037/adb0000422

Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998–2019). Mplus user's guide (8th ed.). Muthén & Muthén.

Oberst, U., Wegmann, E., Stodt, B., Brand, M., & Chamarro, A. (2017). Negative consequences from heavy social networking in adolescents: The mediating role of fear of missing out. Journal of Adolescence, 55, 51–60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2016.12.008

O’Dea, S. (2020). Number of smartphone users worldwide from 2016 to 2021. Statista. https://www.statista.com/statistics/330695/number-of-smartphone-users-worldwide/

Owen, B., Heisterkamp, B., Halfmann, A., & Vorderer, P. (2018). Trait mindfulness and problematic smartphone use. In D. Grimes, Q. Wang, & H. Lin (Eds.), Health psychology research focus: Empirical studies of contemplative practices (pp. 181–205). Nova Science Publishers.

Panova, T., & Carbonell, X. (2018). Is smartphone addiction really an addiction? Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 7(2), 252–259. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.7.2018.49

Parmentier, F. B. R., García-Toro, M., García-Campayo, J., Yañez, A. M., Andrés, P., & Gili, M. (2019). Mindfulness and symptoms of depression and anxiety in the general population: The mediating roles of worry, rumination, reappraisal and suppression. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, Article 506. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00506

Porter, T., Potcovaru, A.-M., Zauskova, A., Rowland, Z., & Grupac, M. (2020). Smartphone addiction risk, anxiety symptom severity, and depression psychopathology. Review of Contemporary Philosophy, 19, 57–63. https://doi.org/10.22381/RCP1920205

R Core Team. (2019). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/

Regan, T., Harris, B., Van Loon, M., Nanavaty, N., Schueler, J., Engler, S., & Fields, S. A. (2020). Does mindfulness reduce the effects of risk factors for problematic smartphone use? Comparing frequency of use versus self-reported addiction. Addictive Behaviors, 108, Article 106435. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2020.106435

Rohsenow, D. J., Abrams, D. B., Monti, P. M., Colby, S. M., Martin, R., & Niaura, R. S. (2003). The Smoking Effects Questionnaire for adult populations: Development and psychometric properties. Addictive Behaviors, 28(7), 1257–1270. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0306-4603(02)00254-X

Rozgonjuk, D., Levine, J. C., Hall, B. J., & Elhai, J. D. (2018). The association between problematic smartphone use, depression and anxiety symptom severity, and objectively measured smartphone use over one week. Computers in Human Behavior, 87, 10–17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2018.05.019

Ryding, F. C., & Kaye, L. K. (2018). “Internet addiction”: A conceptual minefield. International Journal Mental Health Addiction, 16(1), 225–232. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-017-9811-6

Segal, Z. V., Williams, M., & Teasdale, J. D. (2018). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression. Guilford Press.

Shi, D., Maydeu-Olivares, A., & Rosseel, Y. (2020). Assessing fit in ordinal factor analysis models: SRMR vs. RMSEA. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 27(1), 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2019.1611434

Starcevic, V., King, D. L., Delfabbro, P. H., Schimmenti, A., Castro-Calvo, J., Giardina, A., & Billieux, J. (2021). “Diagnostic inflation” will not resolve taxonomical problems in the study of addictive online behaviours. • Commentary on: How to overcome taxonomical problems in the study of Internet use disorders and what to do with “smartphone addiction”? (Montag et al., 2020). Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 9(4), 915–919. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.2020.00083

Stodt, B., Brand, M., Sindermann, C., Wegmann, E., Li, M., Zhou, M., Sha, P., & Montag, C. (2018). Investigating the effect of personality, Internet literacy, and use expectancies in Internet-use disorder: A comparative study between China and Germany. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 15(4), Article 579. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph15040579

Tankovska, H. (2021). Facebook access penetration 2021, by device. Statista. https://www.statista.com/statistics/377808/distribution-of-facebook-users-by-device/

Thomée, S. (2018). Mobile phone use and mental health: A review of the research that takes a psychological perspective on exposure. International Journal Environmental Research and Public Health, 15(12), Article 2692. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph15122692

Van Dam, N. T., van Vugt, M. K., Vago, D. R., Schmalzl, L., Saron, C. D., Olendzki, A., Meissner, T., Lazar, S. W., Kerr, C. E., Gorchov, J., Fox, K. C. R., Field, B. A., Britton, W. B., Brefczynski-Lewis, J. A., & Meyer, D. E. (2018). Mind the hype: A critical evaluation and prescriptive agenda for research on mindfulness and meditation. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13(1), 36–61. https ://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617709589

Van Deursen, A. J. A. M., Bolle, C. L., Hegner, S. M., & Kommers, P. A. M. (2015). Modeling habitual and addictive smartphone behavior: The role of smartphone usage types, emotional intelligence, social stress, self-regulation, age, and gender. Computers in Human Behavior, 45, 411–420. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.12.039

Wegmann, E., & Brand, M. (2016). Internet-communication disorder: It’s a matter of social aspects, coping, and internet-use expectancies. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, Article 1747. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01747

Wegmann, E., & Brand, M. (2019). A narrative overview about psychosocial characteristics as risk factors of a problematic social networks use. Current Addiction Reports, 6(4), 402–409. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40429-019-00286-8

Wegmann, E., Oberst, U., Stodt, B., & Brand, M. (2017). Online-specific fear of missing out and Internet-use expectancies contribute to symptoms of Internet-communication disorder. Addictive Behaviors Reports, 5, 33–42. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.abrep.2017.04.001

Wegmann, E., Stodt, B., Brand, M. (2015). Addictive use of social networking sites can be explained by the interaction of internet use expectancies, internet literacy, and psychopathological symptoms. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 4(3), 155–162. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.4.2015.021

Wu, R., Liu, L.-L., Zhu, H., Su, W.-J., Cao, Z.-Y., Zhong, S.-Y., Liu, X.-H., & Jiang, C.-L. (2019). Brief mindfulness meditation improves emotion processing. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 13, Article 1074. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2019.01074

Yang, J., Fu, X., Liao, X., & Li, Y. (2020). Association of problematic smartphone use with poor sleep quality, depression, and anxiety: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychiatry Research, 284, Article 112686. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2019.112686

Yuan, G., Elhai, J. D., & Hall, B. J. (2021). The influence of depressive symptoms and fear of missing out on severity of problematic smartphone use and Internet gaming disorder among Chinese young adults: A three-wave mediation model. Addictive Behaviors, 112, Article 106648. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2020.106648

Zanon, C., Brenner, R. E., Baptista, M. N., Vogel, D. L., Rubin, M., Al-Darmaki, F. R., Gonçalves, M., Heath, P. J., Liao, H.-Y., Mackenzie, C. S., Topkaya, N., Wade, N. G., & Zlati, A. (2021). Examining the dimensionality, reliability, and invariance of the Depression, Anxiety, and Stress Scale–21 (DASS-21) across eight countries. Assessment, 28(6), 1531–1544. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191119887449

Additional information

First submission received:
January 8, 2021

Revision received:
June 12, 2021

Accepted for publication:
November 18, 2021

Editor in charge:
David Smahel

Full text

Introduction

Smartphones are a prominent mainstay of the modern world, with an estimated 3 billion users worldwide in 2019 (O’Dea, 2020). Concurrent with the rise in smartphone ownership is concern about excessive smartphone engagement that results in functional impairment of daily life (Busch & McCarthy, 2021; Elhai, Levine, et al., 2019). Smartphone overuse is associated with increased mental health symptoms – especially depression and anxiety (Elhai, Levine, et al., 2019). However, only recently have researchers examined mediating variables explaining relations between depression/anxiety and excessive smartphone use.

Excessive smartphone use is often referred to in the literature as problematic smartphone use (PSU), but also “smartphone use disorder,” “smartphone addiction” and similar terms (Montag, 2019; Thomée, 2018). It is important to note that PSU is not currently recognized as a diagnosable disorder. Although similarities in presenting symptoms (e.g., loss of control, withdrawal-like symptoms), and psychosocial risk factors (Billieux, 2012; Billieux et al., 2015; Montag, 2019; Montag et al., 2016; Ryding & Kaye, 2018) exist between PSU and addictive disorders, Panova & Carbonell (2018, see also Starcevic et al., 2020) clarify one does not become addicted to the internet itself and, in the context of smartphone use, the term “addiction” is perhaps best applied to disordered use of internet channels/content accessed via the smartphone. Yet, it has also been suggested that the smartphone itself can become a learned cue related to aforementioned channels/content and, on its own, may elicit cue-reactivity, complicating the term “addiction” in the context of a smartphone (Duke & Montag, 2017a). Moreover, recognizing that the smartphone is a medium that may fuel specific problematic behaviors fits with current research aimed at identifying general risk factors that promote smartphone-mediated problematic behaviors (Billieux et al., 2015; Canale et al., 2021).

To date, no consensus has been reached on relevant criteria to speak of addictive behavior or a “Use Disorder” associated with the smartphone or with the internet, and concerns of over-pathologizing normal behavior remain (e.g., Billieux et al., 2019; Starcevic et al., 2020). Theoretical frameworks describing PSU and problematic internet use (PIU) have generally been developed on the framework of substance use disorders (e.g., Brand et al., 2016; Elhai et al., 2019b; Kardefelt-Winther, 2014), which accounts for conceptual overlap between both PSU and PIU models and dominant substance use disorder models (James & Tunney, 2017; Kardefelt-Winther et al., 2017). Moreover, models of PSU and PIU themselves present important conceptual overlap, yet important differences exist and the uniqueness of each medium has been too often overlooked in past research (Duke & Montag, 2017a; Lachmann et al., 2017). For example, the smartphone’s portability and convenience may contribute to overuse by enabling constant internet access (Elhai et al., 2017), highlighting ease of access as an important factor distinguishing PSU from PIU (Anderson et al., 2017). Differential context of use, such as accessing social networking sites (SNS) only via a smartphone, and preference of use, meaning preference to access certain apps via a smartphone and others through a computer, may also differentiate PSU from PIU (Tankovska, 2021). Montag et al. (2021) concluded that smartphones are the current pre-eminent method for accessing the internet and thus potentially represent the driving force behind PIU, suggesting that investigation of PSU is important and necessary even if questions about taxonomy, construct overlap, and potential diagnostic criteria remain.

Consistent with above information and recommendations by Montag et al. (2021), we use the term “PSU” to capture the predominantly mobile form of PIU and to account for an uncontrolled and excessive use of the smartphone engendering tangible negative consequences, such as interpersonal and school/work impairment (Billieux, 2012; Busch & McCarthy, 2021; Elhai, Levine, et al., 2019; Lee et al., 2019). PSU severity is generally associated with psychopathology, primarily depression (with medium effect sizes) and anxiety severity (with small-to-medium effects) (Busch & McCarthy, 2021; Elhai, Levine, et al., 2019; Elhai, Tiamiyu, & Weeks, 2018; Lewis & Martin, 2020; Porter et al., 2020). PSU is associated with other adverse consequences, such as sleep problems from late night overuse (Yang et al., 2020), and academic interference (Busch & McCarthy, 2021). Additionally, PSU is related to greater distractibility while driving, leading to poorer reaction times and increased risk of driving accidents (Li et al., 2019) and pedestrian accidents (e.g., falls, bumps, collisions; Kim et al., 2017). The predominant view is that mental health problems drive PSU as self-medication, rather than the reverse (Brand et al., 2019). Recent work has considered additional contributing influences on PSU, involving internet-related cognitive biases, and cognitive- and emotion-related coping strategies (see Elhai, Yang, et al., 2019 for a review). The current paper considers two such influences: mindfulness and smartphone use expectancies.

Internet use expectancies represent an internet-related cognitive bias that may contribute to PSU (Brand et al., 2019). Like other areas of overlap between SUDs and PIU/PSU, expectancies are grounded in theoretical frameworks of SUDs and involve beliefs about the positive and negative affective, behavioral, and cognitive effects of using a substance (Cooper et al., 2016) or the internet (e.g., Brand et al., 2016). Expectancies have been consistently associated with SUDs (Montes et al., 2019) and PIU and can be either positive or negative in valence. Positive expectancies reflect the belief that using a substance or the internet results in positive outcomes and/or experiencing positive emotion. However, the definition of negative expectancies depends on the situation. In the context of substance use, negative expectancies refer to perceived negative consequences of using a substance, such as using tobacco results in feeling sick (Rohsenow et al., 2003). By contrast, negative expectancies in the context of smartphone use reflect using a smartphone to avoid negative emotions and experiences (Brand et al., 2014; Elhai, Yang, Dempsey, et al., 2020). Further, positive and negative use expectancies correlate with PIU severity (Brand et al., 2014; Wegmann et al., 2015; Wegmann & Brand, 2016, 2019) and are reinforcing if associated with desired effects of internet use (Brand et al., 2016). Little work has examined relations between smartphone use expectancies with PSU, with one study finding an especially strong relationship between negative smartphone use expectancies (NSE) and PSU severity (Elhai, Yang, Dempsey, et al., 2020). Given the previous discussion on overlap between PIU and PSU, it can be supposed that similar reinforcement processes implicated in PIU can lead to habitual smartphone use (Brand et al., 2019) and consequential PSU (Chen et al., 2019).

An additional emotion-related coping strategy is mindfulness, or mindful attention, involving being open, attentive to, and aware of the present moment and serves as an emotion regulation strategy (Leyland et al., 2019). Mindfulness is conceptualized as regulating emotion through engagement with, rather than suppression or avoidance of, emotional experience (Brown & Ryan, 2003). Further, mindfulness is reduced when an individual engages with automatic or habitual behaviors, such as phone checking (van Deursen et al., 2015). Prior research suggests mindfulness is inversely related to PIU severity (Arslan, 2017; Owen et al., 2018) and overuse of specific online activities such as online games and SNS (Kircaburun et al., 2019) but little work has examined its relationship with PSU (Arpaci, 2021; Elhai, Levine, O’Brien, et al., 2018; Regan et al., 2020). Because emotional avoidance strategies such as rumination and emotional dysregulation are related to PSU severity (Elhai, Tiamiyu, Weeks, et al., 2018; Elhai, Yang, Dempsey, et al., 2020), increased mindful awareness (an emotional regulation skill) should serve as a buffer against the impact of PSU (i.e., decrease the severity of PSU; see Elhai, Levine, O’Brien, et al., 2018) consistent with the overlap between PIU and PSU and emotion regulation theory (Gross, 1998).

Given the prevalence of smartphone use and corresponding benefits (i.e., utility, portability), and consequences of misuse/overuse (e.g., accidents due to distractibility, sleep problems, etc.), it is pertinent to examine potential contributors to PSU. The evidence thus far suggests maladaptive cognitive and emotional coping strategies are associated with increased PSU (Elhai, Yang, et al., 2019). In particular, areas in the literature that warrant additional attention are smartphone use expectancies and mindfulness. Understanding how these variables relate to existing psychopathology within the context of a theoretical framework can provide evidence for the underpinnings of PSU.

Aims

Although the relationship between depression/anxiety and PSU severity has been established (Busch & McCarthy, 2021; Elhai, Levine, et al., 2019), less is known about variables that mediate this relationship – especially cognitions, and cognitive- and emotion-related processes mediating relations between depression/anxiety and PSU severity (see Elhai, Yang, et al., 2019 for a review). We examined relationships between mindfulness (an emotion coping process) and smartphone use expectancies (internet-related cognitions) with PSU severity. We also explored how mindfulness and smartphone use expectancies may mediate relations between depression/anxiety with PSU severity.

Theory

Relevant to this study is Brand et al.’s (2016, 2019) Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model of PIU. I-PACE describes psychological and neurobiological processes and risk factors underlying development and maintenance of PIU. In fact, Brand et al. (2016, 2019) highlighted I-PACE’s application to several behaviors, including excessive use of communications applications, unspecified internet use disorder, and others. First, personal components consist of one’s core characteristics that may predispose an individual to PIU, such as genetic contributions, negative childhood experiences, and psychopathology variables (e.g., depression and social anxiety). Second, I-PACE proposes responses to personal components that involve risk and resilience factors, such as cognitive and attention bias, use expectancies, inhibitory control and craving, and coping strategies. These response variables are conceptualized to moderate or mediate relationships between personal components and PIU (Brand et al., 2019). Finally, I-PACE assumes these response variables influence decisions about using specific internet features or applications, which can lead to maladaptive/problematic use or adaptive use. Although Brand et al. (2016) developed I-PACE specifically to account for specific types of PIU, recent research supports I-PACE in modeling PSU severity (Dempsey et al., 2019; Elhai, Yang, Dempsey, et al., 2020; Elhai, Yang, Fang, et al., 2020; Lemenager et al., 2018; Oberst et al., 2017; Yuan et al., 2021).

Hypotheses

The following hypotheses are posed based on theory and literature presented. The general trend in these hypotheses is that mindfulness and smartphone expectancies represent resilience and risk factors (respectively) from I-PACE, associated with PSU severity, and mediating relations between depression/anxiety and PSU severity.

H1: Mindfulness will be inversely related to PSU severity.

H2: Positive smartphone use expectancies (PSE) will be positively related to PSU severity.

H3: Negative smartphone use expectancies (NSE) will be positively related to PSU severity.

H4: PSE and NSE will mediate relations between depression (H4a) and anxiety (H4b) with PSU severity.

H5: Mindfulness will mediate relations between depression (H5a) and anxiety (H5b) with PSU severity.

Research Model

Our research model is presented in Figure 1.

Figure 1. Hypothesized Model for Psychological Contributors to PSU, Controlling for Sex, Age, and Social Distancing Group.

 

Notes. Circles represent latent variables; squares represent observed variables. ANX=Anxiety; DEP=Depression; PSE=Positive Smartphone Expectancies; NSE=Negative Smartphone Expectancies; MIND=Trait Mindfulness; PSU=Problematic Smartphone Use; Group=Social distancing group.

Depression and anxiety severity are conceptualized to relate to mindfulness, PSE, and NSE, which in turn should relate to PSU severity. Age and sex were also included as covariates of PSU severity; younger age and female sex are associated with increased PSU severity (Csibi et al., 2021; Fischer-Grote et al., 2019). Because data collection was ongoing when the 2020 COVID-19 pandemic emerged in the U.S., we also modeled pre- and post-COVID-19 emergence as a covariate of PSU severity. Our model is consistent with I-PACE by placing psychopathology variables as predictors, protective/risk factors such as mindfulness and expectancies as mediators, and PSU severity as the outcome variable.

Method

Participants and Procedure

The present study used a subset of measures drawn from a larger project. Data were obtained via an online survey through PsychData. The study was approved through the university’s Institutional Review Board. Participants were undergraduates from a Midwestern U.S. university recruited from the Psychology Department’s research pool. Participants completing the survey were awarded course research points. After consenting to an online consent statement, 388 participants participated. We removed 15 participants for duplicate survey entries (based on survey timestamps), and 11 participants for completing a very small proportion of items. We also removed 10 participants for careless/inattentive responding (with a string of 19 or more consecutive identical responses), similar to previous estimates and suggestions (Curran, 2016). The resulting sample included 352 participants.

A majority of participants were female (66.2%; n = 233; sex was coded as: male = 1, female = 2) and average age was 19.79 years old (SD = 3.43; Min = 18; Max = 53; only 3.13% were age 25 and older). Racial breakdown included: 80.7% (n = 284) White, 13.9% (n = 49) Black, 5.7% (n = 20) Asian. Approximately 5.4% (n = 19) of participants identified as Latinx. A majority of student participants were either employed part-time (51.98%; n = 183) or unemployed (40%; n = 141).

A portion of the survey data were collected during the COVID-19 pandemic. We assessed if the pandemic affected our results. The university closed in-person instruction on March 16, 2020, resulting in online-only learning, and significant changes to students’ lives, including vacating dormitories and moving back to their family’s homes. We formed two groups in our dataset: a pre-social distancing group of 169 participants (before university closure), and social distancing group of 183 participants (after closure) based on survey timestamps.

Measures

In addition to demographics assessed, we administered the following surveys. Table 1 displays the current sample’s coefficient alphas.

Depression Anxiety Stress Scales-21 (DASS-21)

We administered the DASS-21 (Gomez et al., 2020). The DASS-21 is composed of three subscales that assess depression, anxiety, and stress. Items are rated via a Likert scale from 0 = Did not apply to me at all to 3 = Applied to me very much, or most of the time. The depression and anxiety subscales specifically were used for the current study and have demonstrated reliability and validity (Zanon et al., 2021).

Smartphone Use Expectancies Scale

Smartphone expectancies were assessed using the Smartphone Use Expectancies Scale (adapted from the Internet Use Expectancies Scale; Brand et al., 2014; Elhai, Yang, Dempsey, et al., 2020). The scale consists of 8 items rated on a Likert scale of 1 = Completely disagree to 6 = Completely agree. Subscales assess positive (e.g., “I use my smartphone to experience pleasure”) and negative expectances (e.g., “I use my smartphone to avoid loneliness”). The scale has adequate psychometric properties (Brand et al., 2014; Elhai, Yang, Dempsey, et al., 2020).

Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS)

Trait mindfulness was measured using the MAAS (Mohsenabadi et al., 2018). The MAAS contains 15 items rated on a Likert scale of 1 = Almost always to 6 = Almost never. The MAAS has demonstrated reliability and validity (Van Dam et al., 2018).

Smartphone Addiction Scale-Short Version (SAS)

PSU severity was measured using the SAS (Harris et al., 2020). The SAS consists of 10 items (e.g., “I missed planned work due to smartphone use”) rated on a scale of 1 = Strongly disagree to 6 = Strongly agree. The SAS has demonstrated reliability and validity (Harris et al., 2020; Lopez-Fernandez, 2017). We voiced all items in the first person for consistency (Duke & Montag, 2017b).

Data Analysis

We used R 4.0.2 (R Core Team, 2019) to clean and pre-process data, and for preliminary analyses. We used the following R packages: careless (for inattentive responding), dplyr (data cleaning), naniar (to assess data missingness), mice (missing data imputation), corrplot (correlations), fmsb (internal consistency), and sjstats (ANOVA effects). Missing data comprised 1.9% of the sample. Data were analyzed using bivariate correlations, and ANOVA to compare scores by sex and social distancing group status.

Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) and structural equation model (SEM) analyses were conducted using Mplus 8 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998-2019). We used weighted least squares estimation with a mean- and variance-adjusted (WLSMV) chi-square, using a polychoric covariance matrix and probit factor loadings (Lei & Shiverdecker, 2020) to test single-factor CFAs of PSU, depression, and anxiety. We correlated residual error variances for PSU items 1 and 2 (involving school or work impairment) and 4 and 5 (both involving psychological withdrawal from lack of smartphone use). We treated the smartphone use expectancies and mindfulness variables as summed scores because of model estimation and convergence problems when adding too many latent variables in our models. For SEM models, we used estimation methods described above.

To test mediation, we computed cross-products of direct effects to obtain indirect effects (Hayes, 2017). We estimated indirect effect standard errors using the Delta method, with 1000 non-parametric bootstrapped replications.

Results

Table 1. Descriptive Statistics (Mean, Standard Deviation), Internal Consistency (Cronbach’s Alpha), and Inferential Statistics (ANOVA) by sex for the Primary Variables.

Variable

Male

Female

ANOVA

M (SD)

α

M (SD)

α

df

Mean Squares

F

p

Eta
Squared

1. Depression

3.82 (3.87)

.87

4.74 (5.04)

.92

1, 350

67.63

3.099

.079

.009

2. Anxiety

2.90 (3.33)

.79

3.85 (4.02)

.82

1, 350

71.90

4.981

.026

.014

3. PSE

15.07 (4.67)

.87

15.31 (4.89)

.85

1, 350

4.86

0.21

.647

.001

4. NSE

12.33 (4.80)

.81

14.41 (5.86)

.91

1, 350

342.3

11.22

.001

.031

5. MIND

58.97 (13.28)

.90

55.20 (14.48)

.91

1, 350

1120.6

5.648

.018

.001

6. PSU

24.51 (8.97)

.85

28.09 (10.17)

.87

1, 350

1014.9

10.6

.001

.029

Note. PSE=Positive smartphone use expectancies; NSE=Negative smartphone use expectancies; MIND=Mindfulness; PSU=Problematic smartphone use.

 

Table 2. Descriptive Statistics, Internal Consistency (Cronbach’s Alpha), and ANOVA Differences by Social Distancing Group on the Primary Variables.

Variable

Pre-social distancing

Social Distancing

ANOVA

M (SD)

α

M (SD)

α

df

F

p

Eta Squared

1. Depression

3.78 (4.53)

.92

5.03 (4.75)

.90

1, 350

6.371

.012

.018

2. Anxiety

3.66 (3.81)

.79

3.40 (3.83)

.84

1, 350

0.401

.527

.001

3. PSE

15.31 (4.84)

.85

15.15 (4.80)

.86

1, 350

0.098

.755

.000

4. NSE

13.92(5.63)

.88

13.51 (5.58)

.89

1, 350

0.467

.495

.001

5. MIND

56.39 (14.61)

.92

56.57 (13.80)

.90

1, 350

0.015

.904

.000

6. PSU

27.72(9.84)

.86

26.09 (9.95)

.87

1, 350

2.379

.124

.007

Note. PSE=Positive smartphone use expectancies; NSE=Negative smartphone use expectancies; MIND=Mindfulness; PSU=Problematic smartphone use.

Correlations among primary (observed) variables are displayed in Figure 2.

Figure 2. Correlation Matrix of Primary Variables.

 

Notes. ANX=Anxiety; DEP=Depression; PSE=Positive Smartphone Expectancies; NSE=Negative Smartphone Expectancies; MIND=Trait Mindfulness; PSU=Problematic Smartphone Use. All correlations were significant at p < .01, except for Age with PSE and ANX with PSE at p < .05, and Age with DEP (p = .402), Age with ANX (p = .579), and Age with MIND (p = .615). Correlations with a darker shade indicate stronger correlations.

CFA and SEM Results

The PSU measurement model showed evidence for adequate fit, WLSMV χ2(33, N = 352) = 158.62, p < .001, CFI = .97, TLI = .95, RMSEA = .10 (90% CI [.09, .12]), SRMR = .04.1 Depression yielded good fit, WLSMV χ2(14, N = 352) = 54.71, p < .001, CFI = .99, TLI = .99, RMSEA = .09 (90% CI [.07, .12]), SRMR = .03. Anxiety primarily showed evidence for good fit, WLSMV χ2(14, N = 352) = 56.05, p < .001, CFI = .98, TLI = .97, RMSEA = .09 (90% CI [.07, .12]), SRMR = .04.

Trait mindfulness showed evidence for adequate fit, WLSMV χ2(90, N = 352) = 343.66, p < .001, CFI = .96, TLI = .95, RMSEA = .09 (90% CI [.08, .10]), SRMR = .04. NSE yielded adequate fit, WLSMV χ2(90, N = 352) = 343.66, p < .001, CFI = .96, TLI = .95, RMSEA = .09 (90% CI [.08, .10]), SRMR = .04. However, PSE resulted in poor fit, WLSMV χ2(24, N = 352) = 5033.83, p < .001, CFI = .24, TLI = .11, RMSEA = .77 (90% CI [.75, .79]), SRMR = .34. In subsequent analyses, we treated mindfulness, PSE and NSE as summed scores because of model estimation and convergence problems when adding too many latent variables.

We tested Figure 1's model, which fit reasonably well based on most indices, WLSMV χ2(393, N = 352) = 743.992, p < .001, CFI = .96, TLI = .96, RMSEA = .05 (90% CI [.045, .056]), SRMR = .09. Figure 3 displays standardized parameter estimates.

Anxiety severity significantly predicted trait mindfulness (negative effect), PSE (positive effect), and NSE (positive effect), when adjusting for age, sex, and social distancing group covariates. Consistent with hypotheses, trait mindfulness (i.e., less awareness; H1), increased PSE (H2), and increased NSE (H3) significantly predicted greater PSU severity (adjusting for covariates). Further, younger age and female sex significantly predicted greater PSU severity.

Figure 3. SEM Model With Standardized Path Coefficients.

 

Notes. Circles represent latent variables; squares represent observed variables. ANX=Anxiety; DEP=Depression; PSE=Positive Smartphone Expectancies; NSE=Negative Smartphone Expectancies; MIND=Trait Mindfulness; PSU=Problematic Smartphone Use; Group=Social distancing group. Standard errors are displayed in parentheses. Sex was coded as: male = 1, female = 2. Group was coded as: participants participating before COVID-19-related university closure = 1, and participants participating after closure = 2. Factor loadings are not displayed for the latent variables, for simplicity, but are available upon request from the first author. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

Mediation Results

Trait mindfulness mediated relations between anxiety and PSU severity (H5b), β = 0.09, SE = 0.04, p = .03, but not between depression and PSU severity (H5a), β = 0.02, SE = 0.03, p = .50. Contrary to hypotheses, PSE did not mediate relations between anxiety and PSU severity (H4b), β = 0.09, SE = 0.13, p = .47, nor between depression and PSU severity (H4a), β = 0.001, SE = 0.13, p = .99. Additionally, NSE did not mediate relations between anxiety and PSU severity (H4b), β = 0.09, SE = 0.10, p = .34, nor between depression and PSU severity (H4a), β = 0.04, SE = 0.10, p = .66.

Discussion

We tested mindfulness and smartphone use expectancies as mediators between depression/anxiety and PSU severity. We focused on mindfulness and smartphone use expectancies as they have rarely been investigated in this relationship. We found that mindfulness mediated relations between anxiety (but not depression) and PSU severity. Smartphone use expectancies did not mediate relations between anxiety/depression and PSU severity.

In support of H1, we found that increased trait mindfulness was associated with reduced PSU severity. This result supports prior findings on mindfulness being inversely related to PIU and addictive involvement in specific online activities, from studies sampling university students from North America (Elhai, Levine, O’Brien, et al., 2018) and Europe (Arslan et al., 2017; Kircaburun et al., 2019), completing internet-based self-report surveys. Mindfulness may reduce PSU by providing adaptive strategies for regulating negative emotion (Leyro et al., 2010), in turn reducing motivation for engaging in PIU/PSU (Kardefelt-Winther, 2017). In fact, better emotion regulation is associated with decreased PSU severity (Elhai, Levine, O’Brien, et al., 2018). Results are also consistent with I-PACE, proposing that emotion processing strategies influence excessive internet use (Brand et al., 2019), as increased mindfulness is associated with increased emotion processing (Wu et al., 2019), thus reducing PSU.

We found that increased PSE and increased NSE predicted greater PSU severity, supporting H2 and H3 respectively. Positive and negative internet use expectancies are related to SUDs in North American adolescents using a repeated measures design (Montes et al., 2019) and PIU severity via cross-sectional self-report surveys in European university students (Stodt et al., 2018; Wegmann et al., 2017), but smartphone use expectancies have been relatively unexamined. We found one study with American college students using self-report surveys (Elhai, Yang, Dempsey, et al., 2020) that investigated smartphone use expectancies, finding increased NSE predicted greater PSU severity. Results are consistent with past findings and also fit with I-PACE’s conceptualization of internet-related cognitive bias influencing PSU. Further, the importance of negative over positive expectancies to PSU is consistent with models of negative reinforcement in the context of addiction (Wegmann & Brand, 2019).

We found that smartphone use expectancies did not mediate relations between depression or anxiety with PSU severity, rejecting H4a and H4b. Prior research found that expectancies mediate relations between psychopathology and PIU severity in a sample of European university students (Wegmann et al., 2017) consistent with I-PACE’s conceptualization of cognitive bias influencing PSU. However, prior work has not explored smartphone use expectancies in such a mediating role. Confounding variables, such as stress related to the COVID-19 pandemic and associated upheavals in academic, occupational, and social pursuits, may have impacted the results. Specifically, expected benefits from smartphone use (and use frequency) may have been different during the pandemic than before (Elhai, Yang, McKay, et al., 2020), confounding the relationship between PSE/NSE with PSU severity. Alternatively, it is possible that smartphone use expectancies behave differently from other use expectancies (e.g., from alcohol). Limited prior work on smartphone use expectancies (Elhai, Yang, Dempsey, et al., 2020) found NSE as a significant correlate of PSU. Future studies should seek to clarify how best to conceptualize PSE/NSE with PSU.

Finally, we discovered that mindfulness mediated the relationship between anxiety with PSU severity, supporting H5b, but not between depression with PSU severity, rejecting H5a. Said plainly, individuals in the current sample anticipated avoiding negative emotion (rather than experiencing positive emotion) by using their smartphone. Increased trait mindfulness may serve as a buffer against the impact of anxiety symptoms on PSU severity. Prior research found that increased mindful attention was related to decreased PSU via online self-report in a sample of North American university students (Regan et al., 2020), and mindfulness mediated relations between depression/anxiety and PSU severity via internet-based self-report in a sample of North American university students (Elhai, Tiamiyu, & Weeks, 2018). Our findings on anxiety are consistent with prior work and with I-PACE’s conceptualization of cognitive and affective responses as mediators between psychopathology symptoms and PSU. Specifically, emotion regulation serves as a buffer between depression/anxiety symptoms and PSU severity. With respect to depression, mindfulness-based therapies for depression (e.g., Segal et al., 2018) highlight the importance of cultivating positive experiences and mindfully engaging with negative experiences. Recent meta-analysis (Goldberg et al., 2018) highlight mindfulness as an effective intervention for anxiety (with medium effect sizes) and depression (with medium effect sizes), but other risk and resilience factors (e.g., worry, rumination, reappraisal, suppression, etc.) may explain the impact of mindfulness on depression and anxiety (Parmentier et al., 2019). Smartphone use expectancies represent another risk/resilience factor and, as discussed above, increased PSE was associated with increased PSU severity. Other aforementioned confounding variables, such as stress related to the COVID-19 pandemic and associated life upheavals, may have also impacted relations between depression and PSU severity (Kiraly et al., 2020). Risk/resilience factors may differentially affect depression and anxiety. Future studies should seek to clarify such relationships.

It is worth noting that female sex and age were associated with PSU severity. This finding is consistent with previous work that women and adolescents/young adults may be at higher risk for PSU (Csibi et al., 2021; Fischer-Grote et al., 2019).

Limitations include use of a sample of university students, which may not generalize to the general population. Self-selected participation may be a source of bias in the data. Furthermore, our data were cross-sectional, and causality cannot be inferred. Additionally, we relied on self-report measures, and self-reported smartphone use does not validate well against objective phone logs (Andrews et al., 2015; Elhai, Levine, Alghraibeh, et al., 2018; Rozgonjuk et al., 2018). We also focused exclusively on smartphone use but did not consider specific activities or the context of smartphone use (i.e., gaming, SNS, etc.). Finally, we did not measure other predisposing or response variables from I-PACE.

Conclusion

Although relations between depression and anxiety with PSU severity are well known, mediators are less established. The current study provides initial insight (albeit based on self-report surveys only in an American sample) demonstrating that positive and negative smartphone use expectancies are related to PSU severity, and supports recent findings linking mindful attention with decreased PSU severity (Elhai, Levine, O’Brien, et al., 2018). Importantly, trait mindfulness mediated relations between anxiety and PSU severity, suggesting that mindfulness may be an important mechanism explaining why some anxious individuals engage in PSU but others do not. Results further our understanding of relations between psychopathology and PSU severity and may have implications for clinical interventions. Mindfulness interventions reduce symptoms of anxiety and depression (Hedman-Lagerlöf et al., 2018) and may be beneficial in treating behavioral addictions (Luberto et al., 2017), which suggests mindfulness interventions, specifically those that increase awareness of smartphone engagement and provide behavior alternatives, may be beneficial for reducing PSU (e.g., Lan et al., 2018). Future research could benefit from further examination of mediating variables tested here, such as mindfulness and smartphone use expectancies in order to further explore mechanisms implicated in why some depressed or anxious individuals excessively engage in smartphone use.

Footnotes

1 Although RMSEA did not evidence good fit with the PSU measurement model, it should be noted that with WLSMV and ordinal data, RMSEA is not as precise when assessing model fit compared to SRMR (Shi et al., 2020).

Conflict of Interest

The Author(s) declare(s) that there is no conflict of interest.

Metrics

2566

Views

1267

PDF views