Understanding the intention to donate online in the Chinese context: The influence of norms and trust

Vol.16,No.1(2022)

Abstract

Due to the advancement of information and communication technologies, online donations have become unprecedentedly convenient, making money received from individual online donations an important form of revenue for many charitable organizations in China. However, factors contributing to people’s online donation intentions, in turn impacting donating behavior, have been under-examined. The current study aims to understand factors influencing online donation intention in the Chinese cultural context by combining constructs from the extended Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB; including the original TPB constructs and moral norm) and trust-related constructs (i.e., trust in charity organizations and trust in technology). The moderation effect of past donation behavior on the relationship between trust and donation intention was also explored. A total of 721 Chinese participants completed the online survey. SPSS was used to perform hierarchical multiple regressions. The results showed that attitude, perceived behavioral control, moral norm, and subjective norm were all positively related to online donation intention. Moral norm was found to be a stronger predictor than subjective norm, raising the amount of explained variance of the original TPB model. Trust in charity organizations was found to positively predict donation intention while trust in technology was not. The results also revealed that past donation behavior moderated the effect of trust in charity organizations on donation intention. This study not only adds to the body of knowledge on charitable donation in the online context by incorporating two trust-related constructs into the extended TPB model, but also highlights the different roles moral and subjective norms play in predicting people’s prosocial behavior in the context of Chinese culture.


Keywords:
online donation; theory of planned behavior; moral norm; subjective norm; trust; past donation behavior; Chinese online philanthropy
Author biographies

Wu Li

School of Media and Communication, Shanghai Jiao Tong University, Shanghai, China

Wu Li, Ph.D., is a professor at the School of Media and Communication of Shanghai Jiao Tong University, China. His research interests cover new media and media psychology. He recently investigates research topics on psychological effects and social impacts of social media use.

Yuanyi Mao

Department of Media and Communication, City University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong SAR, China

Yuanyi Mao, M.A., is a doctoral student in media and communication at City University of Hong Kong, China. His research interests include human-computer interaction and moral psychology, especially the psychological processes and consequences related to the interaction with emerging technologies.

Cong Liu

School of Media and Communication, Shanghai Jiao Tong University, Shanghai, China

Cong Liu, Ph.D., is an associate professor at the School of Media and Communication of Shanghai Jiao Tong University, China. Her research interest lies in quantitative studies in computer-mediated communication, media psychology, and health communication. This is the corresponding author for the article (lcong26@sjtu.edu.cn).

References

Ahn, J.-C., Sura, S., & An, J.-C. (2018). Intention to donate via social network sites (SNSs) A comparison study between Malaysian and South Korean users. Information Technology & People, 31(4), 910–926. https://doi.org/10.1108/ITP-12-2015-0307

Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50(2), 179–211. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T

Ajzen, I. (2002). Constructing a TPB questionnaire: Conceptual and methodological considerations. http://chuang.epage.au.edu.tw/ezfiles/168/1168/attach/20/pta_41176_7688352_57138.pdf

Armitage, C. J., & Conner, M. (2001). Efficacy of the theory of planned behaviour: A meta-analytic review. British Journal of Social Psychology, 40(4), 471–499. https://doi.org/10.1348/014466601164939

Bamberg, S., & Möser, G. (2007). Twenty years after Hines, Hungerford, and Tomera: A new meta-analysis of psycho-social determinants of pro-environmental behaviour. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 27(1), 14–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2006.12.002

Bannister, T. (2013, December 20). The Guo Meimei scandal: Weibo microblogging unveils the Chinese Red Cross. China Development Brief. https://chinadevelopmentbrief.org/reports/the-guo-meimei-scandal-weibo-microblogging-unveils-the-chinese-red-cross/

Bearden, W. O., Netemeyer, R. G., & Teel, J. E. (1989). Measurement of consumer susceptibility to interpersonal influence. Journal of Consumer Research, 15(4), 473–481. https://doi.org/10.1086/209186

Bélanger, F., & Carter, L. (2008). Trust and risk in e-government adoption. The Journal of Strategic Information Systems, 17(2), 165–176. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsis.2007.12.002

Bhattacherjee, A. (2002). Individual trust in online firms: Scale development and initial test. Journal of Management Information Systems, 19(1), 211–241. https://doi.org/10.1080/07421222.2002.11045715

Burgoyne, C. B., Young, B., & Walker, C. M. (2005). Deciding to give to charity: A focus group study in the context of the household economy. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology, 15(5), 383–405. https://doi.org/10.1002/casp.832

Chen, Y., Dai, R., Yao, J., & Li, Y. (2019). Donate time or money? The determinants of donation intention in online crowdfunding. Sustainability, 11(16), Article 4269. https://doi.org/10.3390/su11164269

China Association of Fundraising Professionals (2015). 2014 report on China’s third-party platforms of online donation. http://gongyi.china.com.cn/2015-11/27/content_8410668.htm

Chiu, C.-M., Hsu, M.-H., Lai, H., & Chang, C.-M. (2012). Re-examining the influence of trust on online repeat purchase intention: The moderating role of habit and its antecedents. Decision Support Systems, 53(4), 835–845. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dss.2012.05.021

Choi, S., Kim, H., Chung M., & Lee, S. Y. (2019). Online donation experiences, donation awareness, and intention of future donation among teenagers in South Korea. Journal of Social Service Research, 45(5), 622–633. https://doi.org/10.1080/01488376.2018.1487363

Christian, J., & Abrams, D. (2003). The effects of social identification, norms and attitudes on use of outreach services by homeless people. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology, 13(2), 138–157. https://doi.org/10.1002/casp.719

Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1975). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

Conner, M., & Armitage, C. J. (1998). Extending the theory of planned behavior: A review and avenues for further research. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28(15), 1429–1464. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.1998.tb01685.x

Davies, J., Foxall, G. R., & Pallister, J. (2002). Beyond the intention–behaviour mythology: An integrated model of recycling. Marketing Theory, 2(1), 29–113. https://doi.org/10.1177/1470593102002001645

Duncan, B. (2004). A theory of impact philanthropy. Journal of Public Economics, 88(9–10), 2159–2180. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0047-2727(03)00037-9

Fischer, R., Ferreira, M. C., Assmar, E., Redford, P., Harb, C., Glazer, S., Cheng, B.-S., Jiang, D.-Y., Wong, C. C., Kumar, N., Kärtner, J., Hofer, J., & Achoui, M. (2009). Individualism-collectivism as descriptive norms: Development of a subjective norm approach to culture measurement. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 40(2), 187–213. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022109332738

France, J. L., Kowalsky, J. M., France, C. R., McGlone, S. T., Himawan, L. K., Kessler, D. A., & Shaz, B. H. (2014). Development of common metrics for donation attitude, subjective norm, perceived behavioral control, and intention for the blood donation context. Transfusion, 54(3), 839–847. https://doi.org/10.1111/trf.12471

Gefen, D., Karahanna, E., & Straub, D. W. (2003). Trust and TAM in online shopping: An integrated model. MIS Quarterly, 27(1), 51–90. https://doi.org/10.2307/30036519

Grabner-Kräuter, S. (2004). Building consumer trust in online markets. Springer.

Hofstede, G., Hofstede, G. J. & Minkov, M. (2010). Cultures and organizations: Software of the mind. McGraw-Hill.

Hooper, P., & Stobart, S. (2003). Using third‐party services to reduce the development cost and improve the effectiveness of charity websites. International Journal of Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Marketing, 8(4), 328–336. https://doi.org/10.1002/nvsm.223

Hou, T., Hou, K., Wang, X., & Luo, X. (2021). Why I give money to unknown people? An investigation of online donation and forwarding intention. Electronic Commerce Research and Applications, 47, Article 101055. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.elerap.2021.101055

Hsu, M.-H., Ju, T. L., Yen, C.-H., & Chang, C.-M. (2007). Knowledge sharing behavior in virtual communities: The relationship between trust, self-efficacy, and outcome expectations. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 65(2), 153–169. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijhcs.2006.09.003

Kashif, M., & De Run, E. C. (2015). Money donations intentions among Muslim donors: An extended theory of planned behavior model. International Journal of Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Marketing, 20(1), 84–96. https://doi.org/10.1002/nvsm.1519

Kashif, M., Sarifuddin, S., & Hassan, A. (2015). Charity donation: Intentions and behaviour. Marketing Intelligence & Planning, 33(1), 90–102. https://doi.org/10.1108/MIP-07-2013-0110

Knowles, S. R., Hyde, M. K., & White, K. M. (2012). Predictors of young people’s charitable intentions to donate money: An extended theory of planned behavior perspective. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 42(9), 2096–2110. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2012.00932.x

Kramer, R. M. (2001). Identity and trust in organizations: One anatomy of a productive but problematic relationship. In M. A. Hogg & D. J. Terry (Eds.), Social identity processes in organizational contexts (pp. 167–180). Psychology Press.

Krebs, D. L., & Janicki, M. (2004). The biological foundations of moral norms. In M. Schaller & C. Crandall (Eds.), Psychological foundations of culture (pp. 125–148). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

Kuisma, T., Laukkanen, T., & Hiltunen, M. (2007). Mapping the reasons for resistance to internet banking: A means-end approach. International Journal of Information Management, 27(2), 75–85. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijinfomgt.2006.08.006

Kurland, N. B. (1995). Ethical intentions and the theories of reasoned action and planned behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 25(4), 297–313. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.1995.tb02393.x

Li, Y.-Z., He, T.-L., Song Y.-R., Yang, Z., & Zhou R.-T. (2018). Factors impacting donors’ intention to donate to charitable crowd-funding projects in China: A UTAUT-base model. Information, Communication & Society, 21(3), 404–415. https://doi.org/10.1080/1369118X.2017.1282530

Liu, L., Suh, A., & Wagner, C. (2018). Empathy or perceived credibility? An empirical study on individual donation behavior in charitable crowdfunding. Internet Research, 28(3), 623–651. https://doi.org/10.1108/IntR-06-2017-0240

Lu, B., Fan, W., & Zhou, M. (2016). Social presence, trust, and social commerce purchase intention: An empirical research. Computers in Human Behavior, 56, 225–237. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.11.057

Ma, G. X., & Xie, D. (2015). XIN MEI TI HUAN JING XIA MIN JIAN GONG YI ZU ZHI CHUAN BO NENG LI JIAN SHE: XIAN ZHUANG, FAN SI YU TI SHENG CE LUE [Research on non-governmental organizations’ developing communication abilities under the new media environment: current situation, reflection and promotion strategy]. Press Circles, 6, 36–41.

Manstead, A. S. R. (2000). The role of moral norm in the attitude–behavior relation. In D. J. Terry, & M. A. Hogg (Eds.), Attitudes, behavior, and social context: The role of norms and group membership (pp. 11–30). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

McCole, P., Ramsey, E., & Williams, J. (2010). Trust considerations on attitudes towards online purchasing: The moderating effect of privacy and security concerns. Journal of Business Research, 63(9–10), 1018–1024. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2009.02.025

McKnight, D. H., Choudhury, V., & Kacmar, C. (2002). Developing and validating trust measures for e-commerce: An integrative typology. Information Systems Research, 13(3), 334–359. https://doi.org/10.1287/isre.13.3.334.81

Oosterhof, L., Heuvelman, A., & Peters, O. (2009). Donation to disaster relief campaigns: Underlying social cognitive factors exposed. Evaluation and Program Planning, 32(2), 148–157. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evalprogplan.2008.10.006

Pan, L. (2017). ZHONG GUO CAO GEN GONG YI ZU ZHI HU LIAN WANG SHI YONG YU CHUAN BO SHI ZHENG FEN XI: JI YU SHU ZI HONG GOU SHI JIAO [How grassroots nonprofit organizations in China use Internet: an empirical study from digital divide perspective]. China Youth Study, 10, 57–63.

Park, J., & Stoel, L. (2005). Effect of brand familiarity, experience and information on online apparel purchase. International Journal of Retail & Distribution Management, 33(2), 148–160. https://doi.org/10.1108/09590550510581476

Parker, D., Manstead, A. S. R., & Stradling, S. G. (1995). Extending the theory of planned behaviour: The role of personal norm. British Journal of Social Psychology, 34(2), 127–138. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309.1995.tb01053.x

Pavlou, P. A. (2003). Consumer acceptance of electronic commerce: Integrating trust and risk with the technology acceptance model. International Journal of Electronic Commerce, 7(3), 101–134. https://doi.org/10.1080/10864415.2003.11044275

Pavlou, P. A., Liang, H., & Xue, Y. (2007). Understanding and mitigating uncertainty in online exchange relationships: A principal-agent perspective. MIS Quarterly, 31(1), 105–136. https://doi.org/10.2307/25148783

Pentecost, R., Arli, D., & Thiele, S. (2017). It’s my choice! Investigating barriers to pro-social blood donating behavior. Marketing Intelligence & Planning, 35(2), 243–258. https://doi.org/10.1108/MIP-03-2016-0055

Postmes, T., Spears, R., Sakhel, K., & De Groot, D. (2001). Social influence in computer-mediated communication: The effects of anonymity on group behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(10), 1243–1254. https://doi.org/10.1177/01461672012710001

Rocheleau, C. A. (2013). Organ donation intentions and behaviors: Application and extension of the theory of planned behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 43(1), 201–213. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2012.00998.x

Rose, J., Barton, C., Souza, R., & Platt, J. (2014, February 19). Data privacy by the numbers. Boston Consulting Group. https://www.bcg.com/publications/2014/data-privacy-numbers.aspx

Rousseau, D. M., Sitkin, S. B., Burt, R. S., & Camerer, C. (1998). Not so different after all: A cross-discipline view of trust. Academy of Management Review, 23(3), 393–404. https://doi.org/10.5465/amr.1998.926617

Ryan, R. M., Sheldon, K. M., Kasser, T., & Deci, E. L. (1996). All goals are not created equal: An organismic perspective on the nature of goals and their regulation. In P. M. Gollwitzer, & J. A. Bargh (Eds), The psychology of action (pp. 7–26). Guildford Press.

Sargeant, A., Ford, J. B., & West, D. C. (2006). Perceptual determinants of nonprofit giving behavior. Journal of Business Research, 59(2), 155–165. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2005.04.006

Schwartz, S. H. (1977). Normative influences on altruism. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 10, 221–279. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60358-5

Sheeran, P., Norman, P., & Orbell, S. (1999). Evidence that intentions based on attitudes better predict behaviour than intentions based on subjective norms. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29(2–3), 403–406. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0992(199903/05)29:2/3<403::AID-EJSP942>3.0.CO;2-A

Shier, M. L., & Handy, F. (2012). Understanding online donor behavior: The role of donor characteristics, perceptions of the internet, website and program, and influence from social networks. International Journal of Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Marketing, 17(3), 219–230. https://doi.org/10.1002/nvsm.1425

Siau, K., & Shen, Z. (2003). Building customer trust in mobile commerce. Communications of the ACM, 46(4), 91–94. https://doi.org/10.1145/641205.641211

Smith, J. R., & McSweeney, A. (2007). Charitable giving: The effectiveness of a revised theory of planned behaviour model in predicting donating intentions and behaviour. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology, 17(5), 363–386. https://doi.org/10.1002/casp.906

Stern, P. C., Dietz, T., Abel, T., Guagnano, G. A., & Kalof, L. (1999). A value-belief-norm theory of support for social movements: The case of environmentalism. Human Ecology Review, 6(2), 81–97. https://humanecologyreview.org/pastissues/her62/62sternetal.pdf

Sura, S., Ahn, J., & Lee, O. (2017). Factors influencing intention to donate via social network site (SNS): From Asian’s perspective. Telematics and Informatics, 34(1), 164–176. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tele.2016.04.007

Sutton, S. (1998). Predicting and explaining intentions and behavior: How well are we doing? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28(15), 1317–1338. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.1998.tb01679.x

Teo, T. S. H., Srivastava, S. C., & Jiang, L. (2008). Trust and electronic government success: An empirical study. Journal of Management Information Systems, 25(3), 99–132. https://doi.org/10.2753/MIS0742-1222250303

Tonkiss, F., & Passey, A. (1999). Trust confidence and voluntary organisations: Between values and institutions. Sociology, 33(2), 257–274. https://doi.org/10.1177/S0038038599000164

Treiblmaier, H., & Pollach, I. (2006). A framework for measuring people’s intention to donate online. In PACIS 2006 Proceedings (Article 88). AISeL. https://aisel.aisnet.org/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1061&context=pacis2006

Triandis, H. C., & Gelfand, M. J. (1998). Converging measurement of horizontal and vertical individualism and collectivism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(1), 118–128. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.118

van der Linden, S. (2011). Charitable intent: A moral or social construct? A revised theory of planned behavior model. Current Psychology, 30(4), 355–374. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-011-9122-1

Veludo-de-Oliveira, T. M., Alhaidari, I. S., Yani-de-Soriano, M., & Yousafzai, S. Y. (2017). Comparing the explanatory and predictive power of intention-based theories of personal monetary donation to charitable organizations. VOLUNTAS: International Journal of Voluntary and Nonprofit Organizations, 28(2), 571–593. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11266-016-9690-7

Wang, D. (Ed.). (2011). Development of philanthropy in China: 2001–2011. Beijing Normal University Press.

Warburton, J., & Terry, D. J. (2000). Volunteer decision making by older people: A test of a revised theory of planned behavior. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 22(3), 245–257. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15324834BASP2203_11

Weber, M., & Gerth, H. H. (1953). The Religion of China, Confucianism and Taoism. Philosophy, 28(105), 187–189.

Wilson, C., Van Voorhis, S., & Morgan, B. (2007). Understanding power and rules of thumb for determining sample sizes. Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 3(2), 43–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.20982/tqmp.03.2.p043

Yang, Q., Pang, C., Liu, L., Yen, D. C., & Tarn, J. M. (2015). Exploring consumer perceived risk and trust for online payments: An empirical study in China’s younger generation. Computers in Human Behavior, 50, 9–24. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.03.058

Yang, Y., Wilkinson, M., & Zhang, X. (2016). Beyond the abolition of dual administration: The challenges to NGO governance in 21st century China. Voluntas, 27(5), 2292–2310. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11266-014-9521-7

Yang, Y., Shi, Y., & Zhang, D. (2020). Intergenerational effects on individual charitable donation: An innovative study on philanthropy in China. The Journal of Chinese Sociology, 7, Article 22. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40711-020-00139-2

Yousafzai, S. Y., Pallister, J. G., & Foxall, G. R. (2003). A proposed model of e-trust for electronic banking. Technovation, 23(11), 847–860. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4972(03)00130-5

Zhang, W. (2017). CI SHAN ZU ZHI JUAN ZENG JIN WU NIAN LIAN XU CHAO GUO 500 YI YUAN [The amount of money donated by charitable organizations exceeded 50 billion yuan yearly in the past five years]. http://www.legaldaily.com.cn/index_article/content/2017-09/05/content_7307308.htm?node=5955

Zhong, Z. J. (2015). SHE JIAO MEI TI ZHONG DE GONG YI ZHONG CHOU: WEI GONG YI DE CHOU KUAN NENG LI HE XIN XI TOU MING YAN JIU [Philanthropic crowdfunding on social media: exploring factors shaping the financial success and transparency of online fundraising], Journalism and Communication, 3(8), 68–83.

Zhou, Q., & Zeng, G. (2006). ZHONG GUO CI SHAN JIAN SHE [A brief history of philanthropy in China]. Renmin Publisher.

Additional information

First submission received:
August 8, 2020

Revisions received:
August 17, 2021
November 20, 2021

Accepted for publication:
January 3, 2022

Editor in charge:
Michel Walrave

Full text

Introduction

The development of information and communication technologies (ICT) has promoted the dissemination of information in healthcare, and made online donation more convenient (Hou et al., 2021). Online donation, which refers to an open call for monetary donations through online platforms, has become an increasingly important channel for both donors and beneficiaries in China (Liu et al., 2018). On the one hand, a majority of Chinese charitable organizations have set up online donation channels on their official websites to attract more potential donors. On the other hand, beneficiaries, also known as donation requesters, usually turn to social media platforms such as Sina Micro charities and Shuidichou for help. According to Chaoxi Gu, the vice minister of the Ministry of Civil Affairs of China, money from individuals online can comprise up to 80% of the total revenue for some charitable organizations (Zhang, 2017).

Despite the advancement opportunities the Internet and social media holds for online donation in China, several serious challenges remain. For instance, although the Internet has become an indispensable channel for nonprofit organizations to run fundraising campaigns (Hooper & Stobart, 2003), there is a digital gap among various charitable organizations in terms of Internet use. Due to their limited knowledge about the Internet and its users, many organizations are not proficient in running successful online fundraising campaigns to maintain or attract more people to participate (Ma & Xie, 2015; Pan, 2017). In recent years, there are more and more online charity projects in China that fail to achieve their financing goals within a stipulated period (Chen et al., 2019). Therefore, it is of vital importance to explore and identify the factors which influence people’s online donation intention and provide practical implications regarding how to encourage and maintain their continued online donations in the future as well.

However, factors predicting people’s online donation intention have been under-examined in previous research. The extant literature on donations mainly focus on the traditional offline context, with the Theory of Planned Behavior model (TBP) used as the main theoretical foundation to identify factors affecting individual donation intention or behaviors (Kashif et al., 2015; Knowles et al., 2012; Smith & McSweeney, 2007; van der Linden, 2011). Amongst the limited published literature that explore factors influencing online donation, most lack theoretical guides such as the TPB model, and few touch on trust towards charitable organizations and Internet technology (Ahn et al., 2018; Sura et al., 2017; Treiblmaier & Pollach, 2006). Furthermore, most charity studies have been conducted in the context of Western countries, with fairy little attention given to Asian countries (Choi et al., 2019; Li et al., 2018).

To fill the research gap, the current study combines constructs from the extended TPB model (i.e., the original TPB components and moral norm) and trust-related constructs (i.e., trust in charity organizations and trust in technology) to develop an integrated conceptual model to predict people’s online donation intention. Moreover, to enrich the extant research on donations which has been limited to Western countries, we conducted the current study using Chinese case and samples. Taking into account the potential impact of collectivistic Chinese culture, we compare the different contributions from two types of norms (i.e., subjective norm and moral norm). Furthermore, in order to address the scarcity of research on online donation, two forms of trust (i.e., trust in charity organization and trust in technology) were incorporated into the extended TPB model to gain a holistic understanding of factors influencing people’s donation in the online context. Beyond that, we explore the moderation effect of past donation behavior on the effect of trust in predicting Chinese intentions to donate online, so as to provide more nuanced knowledge on individual decision-making mechanisms concerning online donations.

Literature Review

Extended Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB)

As one of the most frequently cited theories, the TPB (Ajzen, 1991) has been extensively applied to understand and predict human behavior. In its original formulation, the TPB claims that people’s behavioral intention—the immediate proxy of actual behavior—is jointly determined by attitude, subjective norm, and perceived behavioral control (PBC). Several meta-analyses identified that the TPB generally explains about 40% to 50% of the variance in behavioral intention (Armitage & Conner, 2001; Sutton, 1998). To better understand human behavior, especially prosocial behavior, moral norm was incorporated into the original TPB model. A meta-analysis revealed that moral norm predicted an additional 4% of the variance in donation intention after controlling for the original TPB variables (Conner & Armitage, 1998).

Unlike attitude and PBC in the TPB model, subjective norm’s contribution to the TPB model has been debated among researchers, especially when in combination with moral norm in the extended TPB. To some extent, the contributions of subjective and moral norms may largely depend on specific contexts such as the different cultural backgrounds of respondents. Distinguished from that of Western countries, the value of philanthropic giving in China is deeply rooted in Confucian ethics and collectivistic cultural values (Zhou & Zeng, 2006). Therefore, it is necessary to examine the influences of different norms on Chinese prosocial behavior, considering extant studies were mainly conducted among Western people.

Attitude and PBC

According to Ajzen (1991), attitude refers to the degree to which a person has a favorable or unfavorable evaluation of a certain behavior, and PBC concerns the perceived ease or difficulty of performing the behavior. These two variables were repeatedly confirmed to have a consistently positive effect on people’s intention in different types of donation contexts, such as blood donation (France et al., 2014), organ donation (Rocheleau, 2013), and monetary donation (Kashif et al., 2015; Kashif & De Run, 2015; Knowles et al., 2012; Oosterhof & Peters, 2009; Smith & McSweeney, 2007; van der Linden, 2011). In the case of monetary donation, a more recent study among 432 Saudi participants (Veludo-de-Oliveira et al., 2017), attitude and PBC were also found to be significant factors positively influencing individuals’ intention to give monetary donations.

When it comes to online donation, there are at least two crucial differences that distinguish it from offline donations: more potential risks and greater anonymity (Gefen et al., 2003; Postmes et al., 2001). It is thereby necessary to examine whether the effect of attitude and PBC on offline donation would still hold true in the online context. Thus, we hypothesize the following:

H1: Attitude positively predicts individuals’ intention to donate online.

H2: PBC positively predicts individuals’ intention to donate online.

Subjective Norm Versus Moral Norm

Subjective norm emphasizes the perceived social pressure to perform or not to perform a behavior (Ajzen, 1991), whereas moral norm refers to the individual’s personal belief about what is inherently right or wrong to do (Parker et al., 1995). Despite the similarity of subjective norm and moral norm, the two constructs are distinct from several theoretical perspectives.

First, self-determination theory posits that there are two types of motivation, namely, internal (or autonomous) motivation and external (or controlled) motivation (Ryan et al., 1996). By and large, moral norms can be considered an internal motivation, where an individual’s behavioral decisions are self-chosen and completely originate from themselves. In contrast, subjective norms can be regarded as an external motivation, where individuals perform behaviors based on the potential social benefits and consequences from significant others (Sheeran et al., 1999). Second, according to theories on social learning, individuals usually acquire moral standards through interactions with social reference groups and choose certain norms as their own value principles and expectations that guide their behaviors (Krebs & Janicki, 2004; van der Linden, 2011). Although moral norms may have their origin in social or group norms, once becoming internalized and autonomous, they exert influence on an individual’s feelings, intentions, and behaviors independently from social contexts (Manstead, 2000). In short, moral norm focuses on the individual’s intrinsic moral responsibilities, whereas subjective norm resorts to extrinsic motives, such as rewards or punishment from social groups.

Many prominent social-psychological theories are based on the premise that moral norms are the main driver behind prosocial behavior. For instance, both the Norm-Activation Model (Schwartz, 1977) and the Value-Belief Norm (Stern et al., 1999) propose that feelings of personal obligation and moral responsibility lead to the formation of prosocial behavior. To date, support for the use of moral norms in predicting prosocial behavior has been widespread (see Manstead, 2000, for a review). Indeed, the findings of previous studies indicate that moral norms result in the behavioral intention to engage in such prosocial behaviors as donating and volunteering (Burgoyne et al., 2005; Warburton & Terry, 2000). Developing further, some researchers argued that the inclusion of moral norms can even crowd out the significance of subjective norms (e.g., Kurland, 1995). This argument was also well empirically supported. When entering these two types of norms into the same model, studies conducted by van der Linden (2011) in European countries and Knowles et al. (2012) in Australia revealed that moral norm is a powerful predictor of donating intentions while social norm does not play a significant role in the formation of charitable intent.

However, considering the fact that the extant studies were mainly conducted in the context of Western countries, we argued that subject norm cannot be ignored when examining Chinese prosocial behavioral intention due to cultural differences. One of the most frequently documented individual traits is a person’s cultural orientation, which is defined as patterns of assumptions, beliefs, and perceptions that drive people’s attitudes and behavior in society (Hofstede et al., 2010). Among various cultural factors, individualism and collectivism are probably the most useful constructs to assess culture in different countries or areas. Individualism emphasizes self-reliance and places more importance on personal attitudes than on social norms, whereas collectivism emphasizes interdependence and places more importance on social norms than personal attitudes (Triandis & Gelfand, 1998). According to the social norm theory (Schwartz, 1977), people’ performance of prosocial behavior, to some extent, are driven by social norms (such as helping people in need) which are approved, shared and expected by the group or the society. At the same time, prior research indicates that people with a collectivistic orientation have a greater susceptibility to interpersonal influences (Bearden et al., 1989), and are more likely to obey social norms compared to those from Western individualistic societies (Fischer et al., 2009).

Several observations were yielded from the above literature review on the contribution of moral norms and subjective norms in predicting prosocial behavior. On the one hand, moral norm plays an important role in the formation of charitable intentions and adds to the explanatory power of the original TPB model as an independent predictor. On the other hand, the role of subjective norm may vary across cultural contexts. Due to their collectivistic cultural orientation, the prosocial behavioral intention of Chinese people may be positively predicted by subjective norms even when combined with moral norms. In sum, we propose the following three hypotheses:

H3: Subjective norm positively predicts individuals’ intention to donate online among Chinese participants.

H4: Moral norm positively predicts individuals’ intention to donate online among Chinese participants.

H5: Moral norm makes a unique contribution to the original TPB, that is, moral norm significantly increases online donation intention after controlling for the effect of the original TPB variables (i.e., attitude, PBC, and subjective norm).

Trust

Trust is a psychological state that accepts vulnerability based upon positive expectations of the intention or behavior of another (Rousseau et al., 1998). McKnight et al. (2002) extended the concept of trust into the
e-commerce context, and it was divided into trust in vendors and trust in technology (Pavlou, 2003; Siau & Shen, 2003). Similarly, in the context of online government or e-government, researchers conceptualized e-government trust as the trust of the government and technology (Bélanger & Carter, 2008; Teo et al., 2008). In line with the above classifications of trust, Treiblmaier and Pollach (2006) proposed a new framework by claiming that people’s trust in an organization and the Internet would have a bearing on their general attitudes and intention to donate online. We adopt this framework and the classification of trust in our study— that is, trust in charity organizations and trust in (the Internet) technology.

Trust in Charity Organizations

Trust in charity organizations refers to donor perception of the beneficial attributes of charitable service providers (Bélanger & Carter, 2008), such as competence, benevolence, and integrity (Bhattacherjee, 2002). These attributes are key antecedent factors in building initial user trust towards online commercial vendors, which, in turn, affect their transaction intentions (Lu et al., 2016). In the context of donation, Tonkiss and Passey (1999) argued that the extent to which potential donors trust charity organizations are driven by their perception of how wisely an organization has used their donations in the past, an assertion strongly supported by several empirical studies. For instance, Sargeant et al. (2006) analyzed perceptual determinants that influence individual nonprofit giving behavior, and their results showed that trust in charity organizations determined people’s commitment to the organizations, which stimulated their giving behavior. Furthermore, Shier and Handy (2012) found that there was a significantly positive relationship between people’s trust towards nonprofit organizations and their willingness to donate online.

In addition to the findings of the empirical studies mentioned above, we also argue that trust in charity organizations plays an important role in predicting Chinese people’s intention to donate online for two other reasons. First, public trust in charity organizations has been under great pressure in the last decade due to a number of scandals. The media has uncovered several well-known charities, such as the Red Cross of China and the China Youth Development Foundation, misusing their donations (Bannister, 2013; Wang, 2011). These scandals have had a major influence on public trust towards the organizations involved (Y. Yang et al., 2016), decreasing Chinese people’s donation intentions. Second, in line with the tradition of Confucian culture and the moral system, philanthropy in China has been greatly influenced by “familism” (Y. Yang et al., 2020). As a result, when making the donation decisions, Chinese people are inclined to trust those who have a personal relationship (kinship or quasi kinship) rather than strangers, in contrast to the spirit of the “stranger ethic” of modern Western philanthropy (Weber & Gerth, 1953). Given that, in most cases, online donors are not familiar with the recipients, the role of charity organizations is particularly important since they function as a base on which donors can build their trust, especially in the context of collectivist Chinese culture. In light of the above discussion, we propose the sixth hypothesis:

H6: Trust in charity organizations positively predicts individuals’ intention to donate online.

Trust in Technology

Trust in technology is the extent to which the donors trust the competence and security of the Internet (Teo et al., 2008). Although the speed and the convenience of payment transactions over the Internet may seem appealing to the donors, the nature of the medium has potential negative ramifications that could deter them from donating online. In the e-service environment, users may feel threatened by potential risks—such as intangible service providers, loss of money, transaction security, and the leaking of personal information (Gefen et al., 2003; Yousafzai et al., 2003) which, in turn, could make them reluctant to complete e-commerce transactions or even reject using e-services (Kuisma et al., 2007). Among the scarce studies about online donation, Treiblmaier and Pollach (2006) argued that besides trust in charity organizations, trust in Internet technology was another major factor determining people’s intention to donate online. An empirical study by Sura et al. (2017) concluded that Internet technology significantly influenced users’ general attitude toward online donation.

According to McCole et al. (2010) and Yousafzai et al. (2003), security and privacy are the major barriers for people to build trust in an online transaction. However, being members of a collectivist society where people’s self-image is largely defined in terms of “we” rather than “I” (Hofstede et al., 2010), Chinese people are less concerned about privacy issues compared with their Western counterparts. As shown in Rose et al.’s (2014) survey of 10,000 consumers from different countries, the percentage of Chinese who are cautious about the possible leaking of personal information on the Internet is only 50%, which is largely below the global average (76%). In addition, online payment technology in China is very mature, deeply penetrating the daily life of Chinese people. Unlike in earlier stages where only after evaluating the risks of online payment would users start to build up their confidence towards the new technology, Chinese internet users nowadays are much more familiar with online payment and already place a higher degree of trust in it (Q. Yang et al., 2015). Therefore, we are curious whether trust in technology still plays a significant role in predicting Chinese intention to donate online, and propose the following hypothesis:

H7: Trust in technology positively predicts individuals’ intention to donate online.

Moderation Effect of Past Donation Behavior on Trust

Research revealed that perceived uncertainty creates a need for trust-based interaction (Kramer, 2001). In the context of online consumption, trust was found to be an important factor that is capable of deceasing people’s uncertainty (such as perceived information asymmetry, fears of seller opportunism, and information security concerns), which in turn, increase consumers’ purchasing intention (Kramer, 2001; Pavlou et al., 2007). Furthermore, existing studies also showed that previous online shopping experience further promotes people’s intention to purchase online in the future (Pavlou, et al., 2007). In specific, the accumulated online-shopping experience will help reduce the perceived uncertainty about the vendors and increase future purchase intention and behaviors (Grabner-Kräuter, 2004; Park & Stoel, 2005).

In other words, trust and past behavior are two important factors that alleviate uncertainty and promote online purchasing. Therefore, for the consumers who lack previous purchasing experience, their future intention to purchase online would be largely determined by their trust in the online platform and the vendor. Similarly, compared to those who have already experienced online donation, it is plausible that people without past online donating experience will rely more heavily on their trust in the charity organization as well as their trust in technology when making the decisions to donate online. Based on the above, it is hypothesized that:

H8a: The influence of trust in charity organizations on intention to donate online is moderated by past donation behavior.

H8b: The influence of trust in technology on intention to donate online is moderated by past donation behavior.

Based on the above hypotheses, the conceptual framework of this study is depicted in Figure 1.

Figure 1. Research Conceptual Model.

Method

Participants and Procedures

The target population of this study is Chinese Internet users, including those who have no experience with online donation and those who have already engaged in online monetary donation activities. An online survey method was adopted for the present study. The pre-test with a convenience sample of 83 users showed that all the core constructs achieved a satisfactory reliability score (all above .80). Principal component analysis of the seven key variables was conducted using SPSS 23.0. Seven factors were extracted with varimax rotation. The cutoff factor loading score was 0.50. Results showed that the items clearly loaded to the seven constructs and the total variance explained by the seven factors was 87.14%.

Then, the formal online survey was released on WenJuanXing, the most widely used online questionnaire platform in China, in September 2019. The survey link was posted and distributed on the most popular Chinese social network sites, such as Baidu Tieba and Tencent Weibo. At the beginning of the questionnaire, a screening question was used to filter out ineligible respondents who were under 18 years old, as the target group of this study is adults with disposable income.

Table 1. Participants’ Demographic Profiles and Past Donating Behaviors (n = 721).

Gender

  Male

272 (37.7%)

  Female

449 (62.3%)

Age

  18–25

336 (46.6%)

  26–35

181 (25.1%)

  36–45

142 (19.7%)

  46–55

62 (8.6%)

Education level

  Below  Undergraduate

102 (14.7%)

  Undergraduate

439 (60.5%)

  Graduate

180 (24.8%)

Religious affiliation

  No

580 (80.4%)

  Yes

141 (19.6%)

Disposable monthly income (in RMB)

  ≤1800

195 (27.0%)

  1801–3000

116 (16.1%)

  3001–5000

139 (19.3%)

  5001–8000

118 (16.4%)

  8001–12000

64 (8.9%)

  ≥12001

89 (12.3%)

Past donating behaviors

  No

387 (53.4%)

  Yes

338 (46.6%)

Payment channels (among those who had past donating behaviors)

  Alipay

218 (65.3%)

  WeChat

218 (65.3%)

  Online Banking

42 (12.6%)

  Fast payment with bank cards

24 (7.2%)

  Other

28 (8.4%)

Amount of money donated (in RMB; among those who had past donating behaviors)

  <10

70 (20.0%)

  11–50

142 (42.5%)

  51–99

61 (18.3%)

  100–200

47 (14.1%)

  >200

14 (4.2%)

Note. Payment channels and the amount of money donated were calculated among the experienced donors (n = 334).

A total of 721 valid respondents participated in this survey over four weeks. Statisticians have suggested that a researcher would have better power (0.8) to detect a small effect size with approximately 30 participants per variable for regression equations using six or more predictors, if the circumstances allow (Cohen & Cohen, 1975; Wilson et al., 2007). In this study, 14 predictors were included in the regression model, so a minimum sample size of 420 is required to achieve a power of 0.8. Therefore, we can claim that the sample size of 721 yields enough power to draw reliable inferences from the performed hierarchical regression analysis.

As is shown in Table 1, among the 721 respondents, the majority were female (62.3%), young (71.7%; between 18 and 35), had or were pursuing a bachelor’s degree (60.5%), and had no religious affiliation (80.4%). About 62.4% reported that they had less than 5000 yuan of monthly disposable income. Nearly half of the participants (46.3%) responded that they had previous online donation experience. Among these experienced donors, more than half used Alipay (65.3%) and WeChat (65.3%) as payment channels. When being asked the total amount he/she had donated online in the past three months, 42.5% of the respondents reported to have donated 11–50 Yuan while 20.0% have donated less than 10 Yuan and 36.6% have donated more than 50 Yuan.

Measures

Dependent Variable

To measure behavioral intention, participants were asked to self-report on a 5-point scale (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree) with two items regarding their intention to donate online in the next three months. Cronbach’s alpha of this scale was 0.90.

Independent Variables

The independent variables included the original TPB variables (i.e., attitude to online donation, PBC, and subjective norm), one extended TPB variable (i.e., moral norm), and trust (including trust in charity organizations and trust in technology). Attitude towards online donation was assessed with a semantic differential scale (e.g., 1 bad – 5 good, 1 unpleasant – 5 pleasant, 1 worthless – 5 valuable), while other constructs were measured on 5-point Likert scales (1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree). All the scales obtained high reliabilities (Cronbach’s Alphas ranged from 0.83 to 0.91).

Moderator Variable

Past donation behavior was treated as a moderator between the variables of trust and the dependent variable. It is a single item question (Have you ever donated online before?), and participants were asked to choose Yes or No.

Detailed items, statistics, and sources of all constructs are shown in Table 2.

Table 2. Construct Scales, Cronbach’s Alphas and Sources.

Measures

α

Source

Attitude to online donation

.91

Ajzen (2002)

  I believe that participation in online donation would be bad – good

   

  I believe that participation in online donation would be unpleasant – pleasant

   

  I believe that participation in online donation would be worthless – valuable

   

Perceived behavioral control (PBC)

.83

Ajzen (2002)

  It is entirely up to me whether I participate in online donation or not

   

  I am confident that I am able to easily use the technologies involved when donating online

   

  The money required for online donation is affordable for me

   

  Overall speaking, donating online is completely within my control

   

Subjective norm

.86

Ajzen (2002); Smith and McSweeney (2007)

  People closest to me think that I should participate in online donation

 

  People who are important to me think that I should participate in online donation

 

Moral norm

.87

  I am the kind of person who donates money online

 

 

  I would feel guilty if I didn’t participate in online donation

 

 

  I believe I have a moral obligation to donate money online

 

 

  Not participating in online donation goes against my principles

 

 

Trust in charity organizations

.91

Sargeant et al. (2006)

  I would trust the charity organizations to always act in the best interest of the causes

 

 

  I would trust the charity organization to use donated funds appropriately

 

 

  I would trust the charity organization to use fundraising techniques that are appropriate and sensitive

 

 

Trust in technology

.81

Bélanger and Carter (2008); Teo et al. (2008)

  The technology has enough safeguards to make me feel comfortable using platform to transact personal     information

 

 

  I feel assured that legal and technological structures adequately protect me from problems that could arise from    the transaction

 

 

Behavioral intention

.90

Ajzen (2002)

  I intend to donate money online in the next three months

 

 

  I plan to donate money online in the next three months

 

 

Results

Before performing the hierarchical multiple regression, we conducted the Pearson correlation analysis on all the key constructs in our study. As Table 3 shows, behavioral intention was positively and significantly correlated with other independent variables, except trust in technology. The correlation coefficients amongst the independent variables and the moderator were between 0.10 and 0.60, suggesting that all the variables are suitable to be entered into the same regression model.

Table 3. Pearson Correlation Matrix and Descriptive Statistics on All Measured Variables.

Variables

M

SD

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

1. Attitude

3.74

0.79

1

         

 

2. PBC

3.84

0.65

.34**

1

       

 

3. Subjective norm

3.14

0.76

.44**

.17**

1

     

 

4. Moral norm

3.10

0.83

.50**

.17**

.59**

1

   

 

5. Trust in charity organizations

2.03

0.82

−.07

−.25**

.04

−.01

1

 

 

6. Trust in technology

2.47

0.90

.03

−.03

−.01

.02

.57**

1

 

7. Behavioral intention

3.31

0.90

.56**

.34**

.48**

.59**

.05

.10**

1

8. Past donation behaviora

0.46

0.50

.24**

.27**

.19**

.26**

.12**

.16**

.48**

Note. **p < .01; N = 721. a 0 = no, 1 = yes.

Hierarchical multiple regression was conducted to examine the influence of the original TPB variables, moral norm, and two types of trusts on individuals’ intention to donate online in the future. Specifically, demographics (i.e., gender, age, education, income, and religious affiliation) were entered into the first block as control variables, the variables of the original TPB model (i.e., attitude to online donation, PBC, and subjective norm) were entered into the second block, moral norm was entered into the third block, and two trust-related variables, past behavior, and their interaction terms were entered into the fourth block. We standardized the variables of trust in charity organizations, trust in technology, and past behavior, multiplied the corresponding standardized values to create the interaction terms, and then added these new variables to the last block of regression model. The results of the regression analysis for each block were summarized in Table 4, and only the coefficients for the variables that were first entered to the model were presented.

As Table 4 shows, demographic variables accounted for 6.1% of the variance in the intention to donate in the future. Among them, gender (β = .077, p = .039), educational level (β = .080, p = .035), monthly disposable income (β = .126, p = .007), and religious affiliation (β = .138, p < .001) were the significant predictors. However, age had no significant impact on their intention to donate online (β = .067, p = .134). That is, the results indicated that participants of male gender, higher educational level, higher monthly disposable income, and those with religious affiliations were more likely to donate online than their counterparts.

In the second block, attitude (β = .373, p < .001), PBC (β = .150, p < .001) and subjective norm (β = .271, p < .001) were significantly and positively related to the intention to donate online after controlling for the effect of the demographic variables. Thus, H1, H2 and H3 were supported.

In the third block, moral norm (β = .348, p < .001) was found to be a significant predictor of intention to donate online. Furthermore, three variables in the original TPB model jointly explained 36.4% of the variance in people’s intention, and the addition of moral norm significantly increased the variance explained (ΔR2 = 6.8%, ΔF = 96.18, p < .001). H4 and H5, therefore, were also supported.

Results of the last block showed that trust in charity organizations (β = .067, p = .039) positively predicted people’s donating intention, whereas trust in technology did not have any significant effect on their intention to donate online (β = .010, p = .754), after controlling for all the variables in the above blocks. Regarding the moderation effect of past behavior, the interaction term of trust in charity organizations and past behavior (β = −.066, = .031) negatively and significantly predicated people’s donating intention. In contrast, the interaction term of trust in technology and past behavior (β = .008, p = .782) did not predicate their intention. Hence, H6 and H8a were supported, yet neither H7 nor H8b was supported.

Table 4. Hierarchical Regression Predicting Online Donating Intention.

Predictor

R2

β

p

95% CI

Demographics (in Block 1)

.061

 

 

 

  Gendera

 

.077

.039

[.007, .277]

  Age

 

.067

.134

[−.019, .139]

  Education

 

.080

.035

[.006, .174]

  Disposable monthly income

 

.126

.007

[.018, .115]

  Religious affiliationb

 

.138

< .001

[.146, .478]

Original TPB (in Block 2)

.364

 

 

 

  Attitude

 

.373

< .001

[.349, .499]

  PBC

 

.150

< .001

[.124, .290]

  Subjective norm

 

.271

< .001

[.245, .395]

Extended Variable (in Block 3)

.068

 

 

 

  Moral norm

 

.348

< .001

[.300, .450]

Trust and Past Behavior (in Block 4)

.073

 

 

 

  Trust in charity organizations

 

.067

.039

[.003, .118]

  Trust in technology

 

.010

.754

[−.046, .063]

  Past donation behavior

 

.266

< .001

[.190, .288]

  Trust in charity organizations × past behavior

 

−.066

.031

[−.113, −.005]

  Trust in technology × past behavior

 

.008

.782

[−.047, .062]

Total Adjusted R2

.558

 

 

 

Note. N = 721; a 0 = male, 1 = female; b 0 = no, 1 = yes; all the coefficients were standardized.

In sum, the influence of the extended TPB variables (original TPB variables with moral norm) and the trust-related variables of online donation intention, as well as the moderation effect of past donation behavior, is depicted in Figure 2.

Figure 2. Influence of TPB Variables, Two Types of Trust, and the Moderation Effect of Past Behavior on Online Donation Intention.

Note. *p < .05, **p < .01; ***p < .001.

To illustrate the moderation effect of past donation behavior and trust in charity organizations on individuals’ intention to donate online, post-hoc simple slope tests were conducted and the interactive effects were plotted. The simple slope tests indicated that trust in charity organization was positively related to intention to donate online (slope = 0.136, p = .004, 95% CI [0.044, 0.227]) for people without previous donation experience, whereas it had no significant effect on online donation intention (slope = 0.002, p = .969, 95% CI [−0.084, −0.087]) for those with donation experiences. See Figure 3 for the illustrations for the simple slope tests.

Figure 3. Relationship Between Trust in Charity Organizations and Behavioral Intention Among People With and Without Past Donating Experience.

 

Discussion

Drawing upon the extended TPB and trust-related constructs, this study empirically explored the factors affecting Chinese people’s intention to donate online. The results showed that attitude, PBC, subjective norm, and moral norm were positively related to intent to donate online. Particularly, moral norm was found to be a more important factor than subjective norm in predicting intentions to donate online among respondents. Trust in charity organizations was found to positively predict donation intention, while trust in technology was not. The results also revealed that past donation experience moderated the effects of trust in charity organizations on individuals’ intention to donate online. People with no donation experience tended to rely more on their trust in the organizations compared to those who had donated before.

Contributions of Moral Norm and its Comparisons to Subjective Norm

In line with the previous studies (e.g., Bamberg & Möser, 2007; Conner & Armitage, 1998), the present study confirmed that moral norm significantly influences people’s intention to donate online and increased the amount of explained variance of the original TPB model in predicting people’s behavioral intentions. Also, this study revealed that moral norm was a stronger predictor in explaining people’s intention to donate online compared to subjective norm. These results could be explained by the fact that monetary donation is a typical prosocial behavior, which is mainly driven by people’s intrinsic moral responsibilities rather than the rewards or punishment from social groups. In other words, online donation could be regarded as a prosocial behavior that is much more morally loaded than merely being promoted by one’s social/group identity. Another possible explanation for this finding is the context of online channels. Different from the traditional offline donation behaviors featuring high visibility and group membership (Christian & Abrams, 2003; Davies et al., 2002), online donation is usually conducted by Internet users in relatively private or invisible settings. In such cases, the absence of pressure from social expectations and group membership weakens the impact of subjective norm to some extent.

Nevertheless, departing from previous findings which showed subjective norm did not yield significant influence when moral norm was included simultaneously as a predictor (Knowles et al., 2012; van der Linden, 2011), our study indicates that subjective norm is still significant even after the inclusion of moral norm. This conflicting finding may originate from the cultural differences between Chinese and Western people, as we argued previously. Due to their collectivist cultural background, Chinese people highly value being part of a social circuit and take responsibility for the actions and behaviors of people within their group (Hofstede et al., 2010). According to Fischer et al. (2009), individuals within a collectivistic culture are also more strongly guided by group norms, duties, and obligations, in contrast to those who have grown up in an individualistic culture. In this sense, although the influence of subjective norm is weakened in combination with moral norm, it is still a significant predictor of online donation considering the collectivistic mindset of Chinese participants. In addition, there is another explanation for the reason why subjective norm still matters when controlling for moral norm. As a special form of online transaction, online donation involves potential risks (Gefen et al., 2003; Yousafzai et al., 2003), people thereby are inclined to refer to others’ behavior for guidance. Thus, the more they think others engage in online donation, the more likely they are to take the same action.

Influences of Trust in Charity Organizations and Trust in Technology

First of all, the findings show that trust in charity organizations does significantly predict the donation intention but the effect only exists among people who have never donated online before. The non-significant effect of trust in charity organizations among those with past donation experience can be attributed to the transparency and accessibility of information provided by online donation platforms. Compared with the donors in the offline environment, online donors can easily access the status of the donation progress and the distribution of the collected money, which could help to largely reduce their concerns about monetary abuse. In addition, as previous studies have indicated, trust can be obtained and developed gradually over time based on positive outcomes from repeated behaviors (Chiu et al., 2012; Hsu et al., 2007). Consequently, when people donate online and have a positive experience, such as the easy accessibility of information and timely feedback, their perceived uncertainty is reduced, which weakens the impact of trust in charity organizations on future donation intention. That is, trust in charity organizations is no longer a predominant cause of changes in donation intentions for people who have experienced positive online donations.

Another finding on trust worth discussing is the non-significant effect of trust in technology. This result is inconsistent with Sura et al.’s (2017) research which showed that only factors related to Internet technology (other than charity organizations) influenced people’s general attitude toward online donations. As mentioned before, Chinese people usually have a weaker sense of privacy due to their collectivistic orientation and are trusting of online payment methods because of the technology’s maturity in China. These are the possible explanations of the discrepancy between our findings and the previous research results. To be specific, trust in technology is no longer a significant determinant of the online donation intention of Chinese participants due to low levels of privacy concern and the low levels of uncertainty in online payment technologies. Another possible reason why trust in technology was not a significant predictor of intention to donate online is that most scandals in China about online donation (at least those exposed to the public) were caused by the bad management of organizations instead of technology (Bannister, 2013; Wang, 2011). Hence, Chinese people may be less cautious of technology than they are of organizations when it comes to their decision-making of online donation.

Implications and Limitations

Combining the extended TPB model with the trust perspective, the current study sheds light on factors related to Chinese participants’ intentions to donate online. It adds to the body of knowledge on charitable donation in the online context by incorporating two forms of trust into the extended TPB model, and highlights the different contributions of two types of norms in predicting people’s prosocial behavior within the Chinese cultural background. Furthermore, findings on the moderation effect of past donation behavior on trust provides us a deeper understanding of Chinese people’s decision-making process when making online donations.

Apart from its theoretical contributions, this study also provides some practical implications regarding how to increase individuals’ willingness to donate online. For instance, given the importance of moral norm in predicting people’s donating intention, it is of vital importance to show moral recognition for online donation and foster people’s sense of personal responsibility to participate in the charitable activity. Meanwhile, since subjective norm was still found to be a significant predictor after controlling for moral norm, it is also necessary therefore to encourage online donors to share their charitable giving behaviors on social media which can provide a source of invisible social pressure for other potential donors (Zhong, 2015).

Beyond that, there are at least two measures that charitable organizations can employ based on the result that trust in charity organizations significantly predicts behavioral intention only among the people without any previous experience of online donation. On the one hand, charitable organizations should always engage themselves in maintaining a positive image and building their credibility among the public. Specifically, besides conducting external audits, which are generally seen as an efficient means of monitoring (Duncan, 2004), organizations can also make full use of Internet technology to make donation activities (including the amount of money collected and use of the money) more transparent to decease potential donors’ concerns of monetary abuse. On the other hand, charitable organizations can initially appeal to donors by allowing them to donate small amounts money, a lower barrier to entry, and help them to get familiar with the online donation process (including its transparency regarding the use of collected money), which will further enhance their trust in charitable organizations and their online donating intention as well.

Despite the above theoretical and practical contributions, this study has several limitations. First, participants in our study may have varying levels of knowledge of different charity organizations and they may have a certain organization in mind when filling the questionnaire. It could be a confounding factor that is not controlled for in the current study. Although the forms of the third-party charitable crowdfunding platforms in China are rich and diverse, those hosted by major Chinese corporations like Alibaba, Tencent, and Sina were reported to account for more than 86% of the total amount of fund-raising on the third-party platforms, which has become the driving force for the development of China’s online donation (China Association of Fundraising Professionals, 2015). These most popular third-party charitable platforms share many aspects in common in terms of their social media nature and the supported projects. In specific, they all rely on the huge user base from social media services, and are most popular in medical projects for vulnerable groups. Thus, it is believed that the evidence provided by this study is still generalizable to the mainstream charitable industry in China. Nevertheless, future studies should also consider including more variables regarding the specific types of channels/platforms and projects when designing their study measures. Second, our study focuses on individuals’ donation intention in the online context as well as the influence of Chinese culture, which offers us an insightful look into factors influencing online donation among Chinese people. However, such a research endeavor, to some extent, also limits the generalizability of the results. Third, considering the intention-behavior gap, longitudinal studies are needed in the future to better link the relationship between predictors and actual online donation behavior. Finally, the potential dynamic relationship between moral norm and subjective norm remained un-examined in our study, which is worthy of being further explored in future studies.

Acknowledgement

This work was supported by the National Social Science Fund of China [19BXW090]. The authors also thank the financial support from Qixing Charitable Communication Research Project [2021-2022-A06].

Conflict of Interest

The Author(s) declare(s) that there is no conflict of interest.

Metrics

2309

Views

1090

PDF views